Skip to main content
Log in

Reference systems for coding spatial information in normal subjects and a deafferented patient

  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

To produce accurate goal-directed arm movements, subjects must determine the precise location of target object. Position of extracorporeal objects can be determined using: (a) an egocentric frame of reference, in which the target is localized in relation to the position of the body; and/or (b) an allocentric system, in which target position is determined in relation to stable visual landmarks surrounding the target (Bridgeman 1989; Paillard 1991). The present experiment was based on the premise that (a) the presence of a structured visual environment enables the use of an allocentric frame of reference, and (b) the sole presence of a visual target within a homogeneous background forces the registration of the target location by an egocentric system. Normal subjects and a deafferented patient (i.e., with an impaired egocentric system) pointed to visual targets presented in both visual environments to evaluate the efficiency of the two reference systems. For normals, the visual environment conditions did not affect pointing accuracy. However, kinematic parameters were affected by the presence or absence of a structured visual surrounding. For the deafferented patient, the presence of a structured visual environment permitted a decrease in spatial errors when compared with the unstructured surrounding condition (for movements with or without visual feedback of the trajectory). Overall, results support the existence of an egocentric and an allocentric reference system capable of organizing extracorporeal space during arm movements directed toward visual targets.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Acredolo LP (1977) Developmental changes in the ability to coordinate perspectives of a large-scale space. Dev Psychol 13:1–8

    Google Scholar 

  • Acredolo LP (1978) Development of spatial orientation in infancy. Dev Psychol 14:224–234

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersen RA (1987) Inferior parietal lobule function in spatial perception and visuomotor integration. In: Mountcastle VB (ed) Handbook of physiology. The nervous system. American Physiological Society, Bethesda (MA), pp 483–518

    Google Scholar 

  • Bard C, Fleury M, Paillard J (1990) Different patterns in aiming accuracy for head-movers and non-head-movers. J Hum Mov Stud 18:37–48

    Google Scholar 

  • Bard C, Paillard J, Lajoie Y, Fleury M, Teasdale N, Forget R, Lamarre Y (1991) Role of afferent information in the timing of motor commands: a comparative study with a deafferented patient. Neuropsychologia (in press)

  • Beaubaton D, Hay L (1986) Contribution of visual information to feedforward and feedback processes in rapid pointing movements. Hum Mov Sci 5:19–34

    Google Scholar 

  • Biguer B, Jeannerod M, Prablanc C (1982) The coordination of eye, head and arm movements during reaching at a single visual target. Exp Brain Res 46:301–304

    Google Scholar 

  • Bridgeman B (1989) Separate visual representations for perception and for visually guided behavior. Spatial display and spatial instruments. NASA, Moffett Field, Calif. pp 14(1)-14(15)

    Google Scholar 

  • Bridgeman B (1991) Complementary cognitive and motor image processing. In: Obrecht G, Stark L (eds) Presbyopia research: from molecular biology to visual adaptation. Plenum Press, New York, pp 189–198

    Google Scholar 

  • Bridgeman B, Lewis S, Heit G, Nagle M (1979) Relation between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. J Exp Psychol [Hum Percept] 5:692–700

    Google Scholar 

  • Caminiti R, Johnson PB, Urbano A (1990) Making arm movements within different parts of space: dynamic aspects in the primate motor cortex. J Neurosci 10:2039–2058

    Google Scholar 

  • Conti P, Beaubaton D (1980) Role of the structural field and visual reafference in accuracy of pointing movements. Percept Mot Skills 50:239–244

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooke JD, Brown S, Forget R, Lamarre Y (1985) Initial agonist burst duration changes with movement amplitude in a deafferented patient. Exp Brain Res 60:184–187

    Google Scholar 

  • Crossman ERFW, Goodeve PJ (1983) Feedback control of hand-movements and Fitts' law. Q J Exp Psychol [A] 35:251–278

    Google Scholar 

  • Flanders M, Tillery SIH, Soechting JF (1991) Early stages in a sensorimotor transformation. Behav Brain Sci (in press)

  • Forget R, Lamarre Y (1987) Rapid elbow flexion in the absence of proprioceptive and cutaneous feedback. Hum Neurobiol 6:27–37

    Google Scholar 

  • Forget R, Lamarre Y (1990) Anticipatory postural adjustment in the absence of normal peripheral feedback. Brain Res 508:176–179

    Google Scholar 

  • Ghez C, Gordon J, Ghilardi MF, Christakos CN, Cooper SE (1990) Roles of proprioceptive input in the programming of arm trajectories. In: Cold Spring Harbor symposia in quantitative biology, vol. 55. Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press, Cold Spring Harbor, pp 837–847

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg ME, Colby CL, Duhamel J-R (1990) Representation of visuomotor space in the parietal lobe of the monkey. In: Cold Spring Harbor symposia in quantitative biology, vol. 55, Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press, Cold Spring Harbor, pp 729–739

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossberg S, Kuperstein M (1989) Neural dynamics of adaptive sensory-motor control. Pergamon Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Hatze H (1981) The use of optimally regularized Fourier series for estimating higher-order derivatives of noisy biomechanical data. J Biomech 14:13–18

    Google Scholar 

  • Hay L (1990) Developmental changes in eye-hand coordination behaviors: preprogramming versus feedback control. In: Bard C, Fleury M, Hay L (eds) Development of eye-hand coordination across the life span. University of South Carolina Press, Columbia, pp 217–244

    Google Scholar 

  • Honda H (1985) Spatial localization during pursuit-eye-movement conditions: A comparison of perceptual and motor measures. Percept Psychophys 38:41–46

    Google Scholar 

  • Honda H (1990) The extraretinal signal from the pursuit-eye-movement system: its role in the perceptual and the egocentric localization systems. Percept Psychophys 48:509–515

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M (1985) The posterior parietal area as a spatial generator. In: Ingle DJ, Jeannerod M, Lee DN (eds) Brain mechanisms and spatial vision. Martinus Nijhoff, Dordrecht, pp 279–298

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M (1986) The formation of finger grip during prehension. A cortical mediated visuomotor pattern. Behav Brain Res 19:99–116

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M (1988) The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed arm movements. Clarendon Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M (1991a) The interaction of visual and proprioceptive cues in controlling reaching movements. In: Humphrey DR, Freund H-J (eds) Motor control: concepts and issues. Wiley, New York, pp 277–291

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M (1991b) A neurophysiological model for the directional coding of reaching movements. In: Paillard J (ed) Brain and space. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 49–69

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M, Michel F, Prablanc C (1984) The control of hand movements in a case of hemianaesthesia following a parietal lesion. Brain 107:899–920

    Google Scholar 

  • Kesner RP, Farnsworth G, DiMattia BV (1989) Double dissociation of egocentric and allocentric space following medial prefrontal and parietal cortex lesions in the rat. Behav Neurosci 103:956–961

    Google Scholar 

  • Lajoie Y, Paillard J, Teasdale N, Bard C, Fleury M, Forget R, Mamarre Y (1991) Mirror drawing in a deafferented patient and normal subjects: visuo-proprioceptive conflict. Neurology (in press)

  • Larue J, Fleury M, Blouin J, Bard C, Paillard J, Teasdale N, Forget R, Lamarre Y (1991) Arm movement control in a deafferented patient. IBRO, Montréal, August 1991, P39. 18, p 261

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawler KA, Cowey A (1987) On the role of posterior parietal and prefrontal cortex in visuo-spatial perception and attention. Exp Brain Res 65:695–698

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer DE, Abrams RA, Kornblum S, Wright CE, Smith JES (1988) Models for the speed and accuracy of aimed movement. Psychol Rev 95:340–370

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer DE, Smith JE, Kornblum S, Abrams RA, Wright GE (1990) Speed-accuracy tradeoffs in aimed movements: toward a theory of rapid voluntary action. In: Jeannerod M (ed) Attention and performance. XII. Motor representation and control. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 173–226

    Google Scholar 

  • Paillard J (1982) Le corps et ses langages d'espace. In: Jeddi E (ed) Le corps en psychiatrie. Masson, Paris, pp 53–69

    Google Scholar 

  • Paillard J (1987) Cognitive versus sensorimotor encoding of spatial information. In: Ellen P, Thinus-Blanc C (eds) Cognitive processes and spatial orientation in animal and man. Martinus Nijhoff, Dordrecht, pp 1–34

    Google Scholar 

  • Paillard J (1991) Motor and representational framing of space. In: Paillard J (ed) Brain and space. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 163–182

    Google Scholar 

  • Paillard J, Jordan P, Brouchon M (1981) Visual motion cues in prismatic adaptation: evidence of two separate and additive processes. Acta Psychol (Amst) 48:253–270

    Google Scholar 

  • Prablanc C, Echallier JE, Jeannerod M, Komilis E (1979) Optimal response of eye and hand motor systems in pointing at a visual target. II. Static and dynamic visual cues in the control of hand movement. Biol Cybern 35:183–187

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff G (1991) Brain and space: some deductions from the clinical evidence. In: Paillard J (ed) Brain and space. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 237–250

    Google Scholar 

  • Rebok GW (1987) Life-span cognitive development. Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Montréal

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodler S, Euzet JP, Gahery Y, Paillard J (1991) Crossmodal versus intramodal evaluation of the knee joint angle. Hum Mov Sci 10: 689–712

    Google Scholar 

  • Roland PE (1979) Voluntary movements and perception in intraand extrapersonal space. Behav Brain Sci 2:79–80

    Google Scholar 

  • Roland PE, Skinhoj E, Lassen NA, Larsen B (1980) Different cortical areas in man in organization of voluntary movements in extrapersonal space. J Neurophysiol 43:137–150

    Google Scholar 

  • Roll R, Bard C, Paillard J (1986) Head orienting contributes to the directional accuracy of aiming at distant targets. Hum Mov Sci 5:359–371

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (1991) Functions of the primate hippocampus in spatial processing and memory. In: Paillard J (ed) Brain and space. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 353–376

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothwell JC, Traub MM, Day BL, Obeso JA, Thomas PK, Marsden CD (1982) Manual motor performance in a deafferented man. Brain 105:515–542

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanes JN, Mauritz K-H, Dalakas MC, Evarts EV (1985) Motor control in humans with large-fiber sensory neuropathy. Hum Neurobiol 4:101–114

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt RA, Zelaznik HN, Hawkins B, Frank JS, Quinn JT (1979) Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. Psychol Rev 86:415–451

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt RA, Sherwood DE, Zelaznik HN, Leikind BJ (1985) Speed-accuracy trade-offs in motor behavior: theories of impulse variability. In Heuer H, Weinbeck U, Schmidt K-H (eds) Motor behavior: programming, control and acquisition. Springer Berlin Heidelberg New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Soechting JF, Tillery SIH, Flanders M (1990) Transformation from head-to shoulder-centered representation of target direction in arm movements. J Cogn Neurosci 2:32–43

    Google Scholar 

  • Sokal RR, Rohlf FJ (1982) Biometry. The principles and practice of statistics in biological research. Freeman, San Francisco

    Google Scholar 

  • Stark L, Bridgeman B (1983) Role of corollary discharge in space constancy. Percept Psychophys 34:371–380

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein JF (1991) Space and the parietal association areas. In Paillard J (ed) Brain and space. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 185–222

    Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale N, Bard C, Fleury M, Tanoé M, Paillard J, Forget R, Lamarre Y (1991) The production of isometric force in a deafferented patient. IBRO, Montréal, August 1991, P39. 19, p 261

    Google Scholar 

  • Ventre J, Flandrin JM, Jeannerod M (1984) In search of the egocentric reference. A neurophysiological hypothesis. Neuropsychologia 22:797–806

    Google Scholar 

  • Worringham CJ (1991) The integration of “noise” into the structure of movements. In: Requin J, Stelmach GE (eds) Tutorials in motor neuroscience. Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp 443–456

    Google Scholar 

  • Yardley L (1990) Contribution of somatosensory information to perception of the visual vertical with body tilt and rotating visual field. Percept Psychophys 42:131–134

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Blouin, J., Bard, C., Teasdale, N. et al. Reference systems for coding spatial information in normal subjects and a deafferented patient. Exp Brain Res 93, 324–331 (1993). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00228401

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00228401

Key words

Navigation