Skip to main content
Log in

Visual control of flight behaviour in the hoverflySyritta pipiens L.

  • Published:
Journal of comparative physiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

  1. 1.

    The visually guided flight behaviour of groups of male and femaleSyritta pipiens was filmed at 50 f.p.s. and analysed frame by frame. Sometimes the flies cruise around ignoring each other. At other times males but not females track other flies closely, during which the body axis points accurately towards the leading fly.

  2. 2.

    The eyes of males but not females have a forward directed region of enlarged facets where the resolution is 2 to 3 times greater than elsewhere. The inter-ommatidial angle in this “fovea” is 0.6°.

  3. 3.

    Targets outside the fovea are fixated by accurately directed, intermittent, open-loop body saccades. Fixation of moving targets within the fovea is maintained by “continuous” tracking in which the angular position of the target on the retina (Θ e) is continuously translated into the angular velocity of the tracking fly (\(\dot \Phi _p \)) with a latency of roughly 20 ms (\(\dot \Phi _p = k \Theta _e \), wherek≏30 s−1).

  4. 4.

    The tracking fly maintains a roughly constant distance (in the range 5–15 cm) from the target. If the distance between the two flies is more than some set value the fly moves forwards, if it is less the fly moves backwards. The forward or backward velocity (\(\dot F_p \)) increases with the difference (D-D 0) between the actual and desired distance (\(\dot F_p = k^\prime (D - D_0 )\)), wherek′=10 to 20 s−1). It is argued that the fly computes distance by measuring the vertical substense of the target image on the retina.

  5. 5.

    Angular tracking is sometimes, at the tracking fly's choice,supplemented by changes in sideways velocity. The fly predicts a suitable sideways velocity probably on the basis of a running averageΘ e , but not its instantaneous value. Alternatively, when the target is almost stationary, angular tracking may bereplaced by sideways tracking. In this case the sideways velocity (\(\dot S\)) is related toΘ e about 30 ms earlier (\(\dot S_p = k\prime \prime \Theta _e \), wherek″=2.5 cm · s−1 · deg−1), and the angular tracking system is inoperative.

  6. 6.

    When the leading fly settles the tracking fly often moves rapidly sideways in an arc centred on the leading fly. During thesevoluntary sideways movements the male continues to point his head at the target. He does this not by correctingΘ e , which is usually zero, but by predicting the angular velocity needed to maintain fixation. This prediction requires knowledge of both the distance between the flies and the tracking fly's sideways velocity. It is shown that the fly tends to over-estimate distance by about 20%.

  7. 7.

    When two males meet head on during tracking the pursuit may be cut short as a result of vigorous sideways oscillations of both flies. These side-to-side movements are synchronised so that the males move in opposite directions, and the oscillations usually grow in size until the males separate. The angular tracking system is active during “wobbling” and it is shown that to synchronise the two flies the sideways tracking system must also be operative. The combined action of both systems in the two flies leads to instability and so provides a simple way of automatically separating two males.

  8. 8.

    Tracking is probably sexual in function and often culminates in a rapid dart towards the leading fly, after the latter has settled. During these “rapes” the male accelerates continuously at about 500 cm · s−2, turning just before it lands so that it is in the copulatory position. The male rapes flies of either sex indicating that successful copulation involves more trial and error than recognition.

  9. 9.

    During cruising flight the angular velocity of the fly is zero except for brief saccadic turns. There is often a sideways component to flight which means that the body axis is not necessarily in the direction of flight. Changes in flight direction are made either by means of saccades or by adjusting the ratio of sideways to forward velocity (\(\dot S/\dot F\)). Changes in body axis are frequently made without any change in the direction of flight. On these occasions, when the fly makes an angular saccade, it simultaneously adjusts\(\dot S/\dot F\) by an appropriate amount.

  10. 10.

    Flies change course when they approach flowers using the same variety of mechanisms: a series of saccades, adjustments to\(\dot S/\dot F\), or by a mixture of the two.

  11. 11.

    The optomotor response, which tends to prevent rotation except during saccades, is active both during cruising and tracking flight.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barlow, H. B.: The size of ommatidia in apposition eyes. J. exp. Biol.59, 667–674 (1952)

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrós-Pita, J. C., Maldonado, H.: A fovea in the praying mantis eye. II. Some morphological characteristics. Z. vergl. Physiol.67, 79–92 (1970)

    Google Scholar 

  • Braitenberg, V., Hauser-Holschuh, H.: Patterns of projection in the visual system of the fly. II. Quantitative aspects of second order neurons in relation to models of movement perception. Exp. Brain Res.16, 184–209 (1972)

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burkhardt, D., de la Motte, I., Seitz, G.: Physiological optics of the compound eye of the blow fly. In: The functional organization of the compound eye (ed. Bernhard, C. G.), p. 51–62. London: Pergamon Press 1966

    Google Scholar 

  • Camhi, J. M.: Neural mechanisms of response modification in insects. In: Experimental analysis of insect behaviour (ed. Barton Browne, L.), p. 60–86. Berlin-Heidelberg-New York: Springer 1974

    Google Scholar 

  • Collett, T. S., King, A. J.: Vision during flight. In: The compound eye (ed. G. A. Horridge). Oxford: University Press 1975

    Google Scholar 

  • Coggeshall, J. C.: The landing response and visual processing in the milkweed bug,Oncopeltus fasciatus. J. exp. Biol.57, 401–413 (1972)

    Google Scholar 

  • Dietrich, W.: Die Facettenaugen der Dipteren. Z. wiss. Zool.92, 465–539 (1909)

    Google Scholar 

  • Duelli, P.: The relation of astromenotactic and anemomenotactic orientation mechanisms in desert ants, Cataglyphisbicolor (Formicidae, Hymenoptera). In: Information processing in the visual systems of arthropods (ed. Wehner, R.), p. 281–286. Berlin-Heidelberg-New York: Springer 1972

    Google Scholar 

  • Easter, S. S., Johns, P. R., Heckenlively, D.: Horizontal compensatory eye movements in goldfish (Carassius auratus). I. The normal animal. J. comp. Physiol.92, 23–35 (1974)

    Google Scholar 

  • Franceschini, N., Kirschfeld, K.: Les phénomènes de pseudopupille dans l'oeil composé deDrosophila. Kybernetik9, 159–182 (1971)

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frantsevich, L. I., Zolotov, V. V.: Turning dynamics in the phototaxis ofEristalis tenax L. (Diptera, Syrphidae). J. gen. Biol. Akad. Nauk. U.S.S.R.34, 654–660 [in Russian] (1973)

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, A. F.: The saccadic system. In: The control of eye movements (eds. Bach-y-Rita, P., Collins, C.C., Hyde, J. E.), p. 343–362. New York: Academic Press 1971

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaffron, M.: Untersuchungen über das Bewegungssehen bei Libellenlarven, Fliegen und Fischen. Z. vergl. Physiol.20, 299–337 (1934)

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, L. J.: The landing responses of insects. I. The landing response of the flyLucilia sericata and other Calliphorinae. J. exp. Biol.37, 854–878 (1960)

    Google Scholar 

  • Hassenstein, B.: Information and control in the living organism. London: Chapman & Hall 1971

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoyle, G.: Exploration of neuronal mechanisms underlying behavior in insects. In: Neural theory and modelling (ed. Reiss, R. F., p. 346–376. Stanford: Stanford University Press 1964

    Google Scholar 

  • Land, M. F.: Stepping movements made by jumping spiders during turns mediated by the lateral eyes. J. exp. Biol.57, 15–40 (1972)

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Land, M. F.: Head movement of flies during visually guided flight. Nature (Lond.)243, 299–300 (1973)

    Google Scholar 

  • Land, M. F., Collett, T. S.: Chasing behaviour of houseflies (Fannia canicularis). A description and analysis. J. comp. Physiol.89, 331–357 (1974)

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindauer, M.: Behavior of bees under optical learning conditions. In: Processing of optical data by organisms and machines (ed. Reichardt, W.), p. 527–543. New York: Academic Press 1969

    Google Scholar 

  • Maldonado, H., Barrós-Pita, J. C.: A fovea in the praying mantis eye. I. Estimation of the catching distance. Z. vergl. Physiol.67, 58–78 (1970)

    Google Scholar 

  • Mittelstaedt, H.: Prey capture in mantids. In: Recent advances in invertebrate physiology (ed. Scheer, B. T.), p. 51–71. Eugene: University of Oregon Press 1957

    Google Scholar 

  • Mittelstaedt, H.: Basic control patterns of orientational homeostasis. Symp. Soc. exp. Biol.18, 365–385 (1964)

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morasso, P., Bizzi, E., Dichgans, J.: Adjustment of saccade characteristics during head movements. Exp. Brain Res.16, 492–500 (1973)

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parmenter, L.: Behaviour ofSyritta pipiens L. (Dipt., Syrphidae). Entom. Monthly Mag. (U.K.)80, 44 (1944)

    Google Scholar 

  • Poggio, T., Reichardt, W.: A theory of the pattern induced flight orientation of the flyMusca domestica. Kybernetik12, 185–203 (1973)

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reichardt, W.: The insect eye as a model for analysis of uptake, transduction, and processing of optical data in the nervous system. In: The neurosciences, second study program (ed. Schmitt, F. O.), p. 494–511. New York: Rockefeller University Press 1970

    Google Scholar 

  • Reichardt, W., Wenking, H.: Optical detection and fixation of objects by fixed flying flies. Naturwissenschaften56, 424–425 (1969)

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, O. W.: Sexual selection and allied problems in insects. Biol. Rev.2, 298–364 (1927)

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D. A.: The oculomotor control system: a review. Proc. Inst. Elec. Electron. Eng.56, 1632–1639 (1968)

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D. A.: Models of oculomotor neural organization. In: The control of eye movements (eds. Bach-y-Rita, P., Collins, C. C., Hyde, J. E.), p. 519–538. New York: Academic Press 1971

    Google Scholar 

  • Stark, L.: The control system for versional eye movements. In: The control of eye movements (eds. Bach-y-Rita, P., Collins, C. C., Hyde, J. E.), p. 363–428. New York: Academic Press 1971

    Google Scholar 

  • Steinbach, M. J.: Eye tracking of self-moved targets: the role of efference. J. exp. Psychol.82, 366–376 (1969)

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strausfeld, N. J.: Golgi studies on insects. II. The optic lobes of Diptera. Phil. Trans. B258, 135–223 (1970)

    Google Scholar 

  • Strausfeld, N. J.: The organization of the insect visual system (light microscopy). I. Projections and arrangements of neurons in the lamina ganglionaris of Diptera. Z. Zellforsch.121, 377–441 (1971)

    Google Scholar 

  • Tansley, K.: Vision in vertebrates. London: Chapman & Hall 1965

    Google Scholar 

  • Virsik, R., Reichardt, W.: Tracking of moving objects by the flyMusca domestica. Naturwissenschaften61, 132–133 (1974)

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, G.: Verhaltensphysiologische Untersuchungen über die den Weibchenbesprung des Stubenfliegen-Männchens (Musca domestica) auslösenden optischen Faktoren. Z. Tierpsychol.14, 309–323 (1957)

    Google Scholar 

  • Walls, G. L.: The vertebrate eye. New York: Haffner Publishing Co. 1963

    Google Scholar 

  • Wehner, R., Flatt, I.: The visual orientation of desert ants,Cataglyphis bicolor, by means of terrestrial cues. In: Information processing in the visual systems of arthropods (ed. Wehner, R.), p. 295–302. Berlin-Heidelberg-New York: Springer 1972

    Google Scholar 

  • Weis-Fogh, T.: Quick estimates of flight fitness in hovering animals, including novel mechanisms for lift production. J. exp. Biol.59, 169–230 (1973)

    Google Scholar 

  • Westheimer, G., Conover, D. W.: Smooth eye movements in the absence of a moving visual stimulus. J. exp. Psychol.47, 283–284 (1954)

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wickler, W., Seibt, U.: Zur Ethologie afrikanischer Stielaugenfliegen (Diptera, Diopsidae). Z. Tierpsychol.31, 113–130 (1972)

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. M.: Inherent asymmetry and reflex modulation of the locust flight motor pattern. J. exp. Biol.48, 631–641 (1968)

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. M., Hoy, R. R.: Optomotor reaction, locomotory bias and reactive inhibition in the milkweed bugOncopeltus and the beetleZophobas. Z. vergl. Physiol.58, 136–152 (1968)

    Google Scholar 

  • Yarbus, A. L.: Eye movements and vision. New York: Plenum Press 1967

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, L. R.: Pursuit eye tracking movements. In: The control of eye movements (ed. Bach-y-Rita, P., Collins, C. C., Hyde, J. E.), p. 429–443. New York: Academic Press 1971

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

We are very grateful to Richard Andrew and Peter Slater for critically reading the manuscript, and to Alan King for helpful discussion. We thank the U.K. Science Research Council for financial support.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Collett, T.S., Land, M.F. Visual control of flight behaviour in the hoverflySyritta pipiens L.. J. Comp. Physiol. 99, 1–66 (1975). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01464710

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01464710

Keywords

Navigation