Introduction

Impulsivity can be defined as a tendency towards unplanned actions, and is widely implicated in the development and maintenance of addictive behaviours (Verdejo-Garcia et al. 2008). As a personality trait, impulsivity is assessed using self-report questionnaires such as the Barratt Impulsivity Scale (BIS) (Patton et al. 1995), and these measures detect elevated impulsivity in regular users of various substances including stimulants, opiates, alcohol and Ecstasy (Verdejo-Garcia et al. 2008). At a neuropsychological level, impulsivity is thought to arise from an impairment of inhibitory control. This is a core component of executive function that is implemented by a network of cortical and subcortical structures including the lateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) (Aron et al. 2004; Jentsch and Taylor 1999; Lyvers 2000). One widely used neuropsychological probe of inhibitory control is the stop-signal test (Logan 1994), which assesses the ability to over-ride a prepotent “Go” response upon presentation of an infrequent stop signal (an auditory beep). The task is highly sensitive to inhibitory deficits, because the difficulty of stopping can be adjusted for each subject by manipulating the delay between the Go stimulus and the stop signal. This allows estimation of a stop-signal reaction time (SSRT), which indexes the efficiency of the stopping process. In addition, the task can be used to assess basic psychomotor speed and behavioral adjustment processes, such as the tendency to slow responding following an error (Rabbitt 1966).

The stop-signal test has been used in several studies of stimulant users, who displayed a selective deficit in inhibitory function (i.e. slower SSRTs) in the presence of intact psychomotor speed (go reaction time) (Fillmore and Rush 2002; Monterosso et al. 2005). Abstinent cocaine users were also reported to show attenuated slowing of reaction time following stop-signal trials (Li et al. 2006), which may indicate a deficit in performance monitoring mediated by the anterior cingulate cortex (Kaufman et al. 2003; Kerns et al. 2004; Li et al. 2009). The findings of impaired SSRT converge with other reports of impulsivity in stimulant users using alternative paradigms (Clark et al. 2006; Kirby and Petry 2004; Leland and Paulus 2005; Quednow et al. 2007; Salo et al. 2002). Neuropsychological measures of inhibitory control have been less widely studied in alcohol dependence, and findings are less consistent with regard to both response inhibition (e.g. Go-No Go task; Bjork et al. 2004; Duka et al. 2003; Mitchell et al. 2005; Rose and Duka 2008) and delay discounting (the tendency to choose small immediate rewards over larger delayed rewards; Bjork et al. 2004; Dom et al. 2006; Kirby and Petry 2004; Mitchell et al. 2005). The first aim of the present study was to thoroughly characterise performance on the stop-signal test in a group of outpatients with alcohol dependence.

The observations of impulsivity in addicted groups may arise through two possible routes, which are not mutually exclusive. Impulsivity may occur as a consequence of prolonged substance consumption, or may predate the onset of the addiction, associated with the vulnerability to addictive disorders (Verdejo-Garcia et al. 2008). It is difficult to separate these pathways in studies in current users. The second aim of the present study was to compare impulsivity and inhibitory control in alcohol dependence against a second group of problem gamblers. Problem gambling is a putative behavioural addiction that is thought to share aetiological overlap with substance use disorders including alcohol dependence (Petry et al. 2005; Potenza 2006; Slutske et al. 2000). Any progressive deterioration associated with chronic drug consumption is assumed to be minimal in problem gamblers. We reasoned that a shared deficit across both the alcohol-dependent and problem gambler groups would implicate a vulnerability marker, whereas an impairment that was selective to alcohol dependence would implicate a substance-related deterioration.

In addition to the neuropsychological probe of response inhibition, we administered the BIS (Patton et al. 1995), and two clinical self-report measures of relevance to problem gambling. Adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is highly co-morbid with problem gambling (Breyer et al. 2009; Carlton and Manowitz 1992; Specker et al. 1995), and impulsivity is regarded as a central deficit in ADHD (Barkley 1997; Nigg 2001). Children and adults with ADHD are robustly impaired on the stop-signal test (Lijffijt et al. 2005). We assessed ADHD symptoms using the ADHD Self Report Scale (ASRS) (Kessler et al. 2005), which has not previously been used in research studies of problem gambling. In addition, we aimed to assess compulsive tendencies using the Padua Inventory (Washington State University Revision; Burns et al. 1996), a measure of sub-clinical obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) symptoms. Some researchers have conceptualised problem gambling and other DSM impulse control disorders as lying along an ‘impulsive-compulsive’ spectrum, with OCD identified as the prototypical compulsive condition (Grant and Potenza 2006; Hollander and Wong 1995; McElroy et al. 1994). Thus, an ancillary aim of the present study was to explore the presence of these co-morbidities in a non-clinical group of problem gamblers recruited in the community and to compare the clinical ratings from this putative behavioural addiction against a known substance addiction (alcohol dependence).

Methods

Participants

Three groups of participants were recruited: outpatients with alcohol dependence (n = 23), subjects with problem gambling or probable pathological gambling (n = 21) and healthy controls (n = 27). All subjects were male. Alcohol dependent individuals were recruited from treatment centres in Southend-on-Sea and Cambridge, UK, with DSM-IV diagnoses confirmed in a semi-structured interview with a psychiatrist (J.L., N.A.B.). The degree of dependency was assessed with the Severity of Alcohol Dependence Questionnaire (SADQ; Stockwell et al. 1979). Age of onset of problem drinking ranged from 11 to 35 years (21.9 ± 7.6) and duration of problem drinking, from 4.0 to 38.7 years (17.2 ± 9.9). Eleven patients in the alcohol-dependent group were taking medication (disulfiram, n = 3; antidepressants, n = 7; both, n = 1). Sobriety at time of testing was confirmed by breath alcohol readings <0.01 mg/L (Lion Alcometer S-D2; Lion Laboratories Ltd., Barry, UK). Four subjects had consumed alcohol in the past 48 h (but were sober at testing), and all others were abstinent for >1 week, with 16 in remission.

Problem gamblers were recruited through community advertisement in Cambridge, UK and the GamCare website (www.gamcare.org.uk). All subjects scored ≥3 on the South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS) (Lesieur and Blume 1987), indicative of ‘problem gambling’, and 16 (76%) met the more stringent criteria for ‘probable pathological gambling’ (SOGS ≥ 5). Healthy controls were recruited through community advertisement and from a panel of research volunteers. Controls and alcohol-dependent subjects scored ≤2 on the SOGS. Exclusion criteria for all groups were: age over 65, co-morbid psychiatric illness, history of head injury, or neurological disorder, assessed by means of a locally developed screening questionnaire. The protocol was approved by the Cambridge Research Ethics Committee and all volunteers provided written informed consent.

Participants completed a number of self-report questionnaires to assess impulsivity and related clinical constructs: (1) Barratt Impulsivity Scale (version 11; Patton et al. 1995), a 30-item scale comprising three subscales (attentional, motor and non-planning) was used to assess trait impulsivity;( 2) Padua OCD Inventory, Washington State University Revision (Burns et al. 1996), a 39-item scale with five subscales (contamination/washing, dressing/grooming, checking, thoughts of harm, impulses to harm), that indexes sub-clinical OCD behaviour; (3) ASRS Symptom Checklist (Kessler et al. 2005) to index symptoms of adult ADHD.

Stop-signal test

Subjects completed the stop-signal test as part of a neuropsychological assessment, with the other tasks reported separately (Lawrence et al. 2009). The stop-signal test took around 15 min to complete, on a PC computer using a two-button response box. On Go trials, the subject made a rapid two-choice button presses to left- or right- facing arrow stimuli (i.e. left arrow = left button, right arrow = right button). On a minority of trials (25%), an auditory stop signal (a 300 Hz tone) followed the presentation of the arrow (stop trials). Subjects were instructed to attempt to inhibit their Go response on hearing the stop signal. The difficulty of stopping was varied by adjusting the delay between the onset of the Go signal and the stop signal, using a tracking staircase function with 50 ms steps: a failed stop trial reduced the subsequent delay by 50 ms and a successful stop increased the delay by 50 ms. This method converges upon a stop-signal delay (SSD) at which the subject successfully stops on half the trials (the SSD50). The SSRT can be calculated by subtracting the SSD50 from the median go reaction time. The tracking procedure provides a superior estimation of the SSRT than blocked SSD procedures (Band et al. 2003).

Subjects completed five blocks of 64 trials, following a 16-trial practice of Go-only trials. Data from one problem gambler were lost due to software failure. Data were screened for outliers and to confirm convergence of the tracking function (Clark et al. 2005). Seven subjects (two controls, four alcohol-dependent, one problem gambler) failed to achieve convergence, either through too high (>60%) or too low (<40%) levels of successful inhibition. These staircase failures may arise through strategic slowing of the go reaction time, or through inconsistent performance or excessive distraction; they invalidate an assumption of the horserace model that Go- and stop-related processes are independent (Logan 1994). As a consequence, these subjects were excluded from analyses. The final groups for analysis (with inhibition rates approximating 50%) were 19 alcohol-dependent, 19 problem gamblers and 25 healthy controls.

Dependent variables were the SSRT, the median Go reaction time, and the total number of Go discrimination errors (e.g. a left button press to a right-facing arrow). In addition, post-error behaviour was analysed following Li et al. (2008): go reaction times were calculated for successful (non-error) Go trials in three conditions: (1) following Go trials (pG), (2) following successful stop trials (pSS), 3) following failed stop trials (pFS). Given that the tracking algorithm aims to identify the SSD50, approximately equal numbers of pSS and pFS trials were expected. The rate of Go discrimination errors was too low to allow analysis of adjustment following Go errors.

Statistical analysis

Data were analysed with Statistical Package for the Social Sciences v15 (SPSS; Chicago, Illinois) using the principles set out in Cardinal and Aitken (2006) and Howell (2007). Behavioural data meeting assumptions of normality were analysed with analysis of variance (ANOVA), with two-tailed tests thresholded at p < .05. Where ANOVA models contained a within-subjects variable with >2 levels, Mauchly's test of sphericity of the covariance matrix was applied. Where terms violated the sphericity assumption, degrees of freedom were corrected using Greenhouse-Geisser's epsilon (ε is reported where this correction has been applied). Post-hoc investigation of effects of group was conducted using Fisher's LSD, as is appropriate for designs with three groups. Post-hoc differences in trial type used the SPSS ‘simple’ contrast with go reaction time as the control condition, adjusted for multiple comparisons using the Dunn-Sidak correction. Associations were tested using Kendall's Tau-b concordance coefficients to minimise the influence of outliers on the small sample sizes

Results

Demographic, clinical and personality data are reported in Table 1. There were no significant differences in age or years of education, although the ANOVA for age approached significance (F(2, 68) = 2.68, p = 0.076). The alcohol-dependent group were significantly older than the problem gamblers (p = 0.025), but importantly, neither target group differed significantly from the healthy controls.

Table 1 Demographic and self-report data in the alcohol-dependent (AD), problem gambler (PG) and healthy control (HC) groups. Values indicate mean (SD)

On the BIS, there was a significant between-group difference on the total score and each of the subscales, and in each case, both the problem gamblers and alcohol-dependent groups were significantly elevated compared with controls (see Table 1). On the BIS total, the problem gamblers also scored significantly higher than the alcohol-dependent group (p = .048). Whilst there was no significant difference on the Padua Inventory total score, there was a significant effect on the checking subscale (F(2, 68) = 4.15, p = 0.018), with significantly higher scores in the alcohol-dependent group compared with controls (p = .007) and marginally higher scores in the problem gamblers compared with controls (p = .060). On the ASRS, there was a significant group difference (F(2, 68) = 4.54, p = 0.014), with elevated scores in both the alcohol-dependent group (p = 0.017) and problem gamblers (p = 0.009) relative to controls, and no difference between the target groups (p = 0.780). A further analysis of the ASRS data applied a threshold of ≥14 as indicative of a likely diagnosis of adult ADHD (Kessler et al. 2007). More subjects exceeded this threshold in the alcohol-dependent (30%) and problem gambler (38%) groups than in the healthy controls (7%; χ 2 = 7.28, df = 2, p = 0.022).

Stop-signal task

In the restricted sample used in the analysis of the stop-signal test, there was a fully significant group difference in age (F(2, 60) = 4.19, p = 0.020) but no difference in years of education (F(2, 60) = 1.70, p = 0.191). Age was correlated positively with both go reaction time (T(63) = 0.412, p < 0.0001) and SSRT (T(63) = 0.245, p = 0.005) in the overall sample, and was thus employed as a covariate in the ANOVA models. ANOVA revealed a significant difference in go reaction time (F(2, 59) = 8.08, p = 0.001), due to slower responding in the alcohol-dependent group compared with controls (p < 0.0001) and problem gamblers (p = 0.063; see Table 2). Whilst the tracking function ensured that the probability of inhibition converged at approximately 50% in each group (alcohol-dependent = 48%, problem gamblers = 47%, healthy controls = 45%), there was a small difference in P(inhib) across groups (F(2, 59) = 2.73, p = 0.073), and hence P(inhib) was included as a covariate (along with age) in the ANOVA of SSRT. The effect of group on SSRT was significant \( \left( {F\left( {2,58} \right) = 3.82, p = 0.028} \right) \), such that the alcohol-dependent group had significantly longer SSRTs than controls (p = 0.008). Within the alcohol-dependent group, there were no differences in Go RT or SSRT as a function of medication status (both t < 0.725, p > 0.10). Could the deficit in go reaction time in the alcohol-dependent group have arisen as strategic slowing in response to the stop signals? Three effects indicate that this is unlikely. First, analysis of reaction time data from the practice block (before introduction of the stop signal) confirmed psychomotor slowing in the alcohol-dependent group compared with controls (p = .005). Second, in the alcohol-dependent group, there was no change in reaction time between the practice block and main task (t(18) = 0.128, p = 0.899). Third, go reaction time and SSRT did not correlate within any group \( \left( {{\text{Tau}} = - 0.07 \,{\text{to}}\, - 0.17, p > 0.23} \right) \).

Table 2 Performance on the stop-signal test in the alcohol-dependent (AD), problem gambler (PG) and healthy control (HC) groups, covarying for age. Values indicate mean (SD)

In the analysis of post-signal slowing, ANOVA of the three trial types (pG, pSS and pFS) revealed the expected significant effect of group (F(2, 59) = 6.74, p = 0.002) due to overall slowing in the alcohol-dependent group. The main effect of trial type was non-significant (F(1.8, 103.8) = 0.398, p = 0.646, ε = 0.88) but there was a significant trial type by group interaction (F(3.5, 103.8) = 2.59, p = 0.048, ε = 0.88). The interaction term (see Fig. 1) was driven by differences between the alcohol-dependent and healthy control groups (group x trial type: F(2, 82) = 4.85, p = 0.011). These groups differed in their responses following failed stops (F(1, 41) = 6.71, p = 0.013) but not following successful stops (F(1, 41) = 0.004, p = 0.951). Whilst controls showed significant slowing on pFS trials (t(24) = 3.31, p = 0.004), the alcohol-dependent group showed a non-significant tendency to speed up on pFS trials (see Fig. 1; t(18) = 1.07, p = 0.3). The group by trial-type interaction terms were not significant in the other paired ANOVAs of problem gamblers versus controls (F(1.6, 66.2) = 1.40, p = 0.252, ε = 0.81) or gamblers versus alcohol dependence (F(1.6, 57.0) = 0.482, p = 0.581, ε = 0.82). Thus, performance in the problem gamblers fell between the control and alcohol-dependent groups and did not differ significantly from either.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The ordinate displays the difference in reaction time on successful and failed inhibitions, compared with a baseline of reaction time following a Go trial. There is a significant group by trial-type interaction due to abolished slowing after failed inhibitions in the alcohol-dependent (AD) group, compared with the healthy controls (HC). Problem gamblers (PG) do not differ significantly from either group. Error Bars represent ±1 S.E.M

In addition, we ran two sets of exploratory correlational analyses. Given the impairment on the stop-signal test in the alcohol-dependent group, we looked at the associations between stop-signal variables (SSRT, go reaction time, post-stop slowing) and clinical measures of alcohol dependence (SADQ score, age of onset, duration, length of abstinence) in the alcohol-dependent group. There was a positive association between go reaction time and duration of alcohol abuse (Τ(19) = 0.40, p = 0.016), such that greater chronicity was associated with slower psychomotor function (see Fig. 2a). This association was not obviously explained by age (that more chronic cases tended to be older) or protracted withdrawal symptoms, as go reaction time was not significantly related to these variables (age T(19) = 0.235, p = .161; days abstinent T(19) = −047, p = 0.779). There was a negative association between slowing following a failed inhibition and the SADQ score (Τ(19) = −0.38, p = 0.023), such that more severe dependence was associated with a speeding (rather than a slowing) of response speed following failed stop trials (see Fig. 2b). Second, we looked for associations between the self-report measures of impulsivity (BIS total, ASRS) and the three performance indices on the stop-signal test, in the collapsed sample (n = 63). Whilst the two self-report measures were related (T = 0.370, p < 0.0001), no associations with stopping performance were significant \( \left( {{\text{Tau}} = - 0.06 \,{\text{to}}\, - 0.10, p > 0.27} \right) \).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Clinical predictors of task performance. a Slowed go reaction time (Go RT) in the alcohol-dependent group was associated with greater duration of alcohol use (left). b Diminished post-error slowing (i.e. slowing after failures to stop on a stop-signal trial) in the alcohol-dependent group was associated with greater score on the Severity of Alcohol Dependence Questionnaire (SADQ)

Discussion

Individuals with alcohol dependence and problem gambling were compared on the stop-signal test, and additionally completed self-report measures of impulsivity. The questionnaire data indicated significant elevations in trait impulsivity (BIS) and adult ADHD symptoms (ASRS) in both target groups compared with healthy controls, consistent with the central role of impulsivity in current theoretical models of addiction (Goldstein and Volkow 2002; Jentsch and Taylor 1999). In contrast, the neurocognitive measure of response inhibition only detected a significant deficit in the alcohol-dependent group, who were impaired on multiple components of the task. The alcohol-dependent group displayed slower SSRTs, in the context of overall psychomotor slowing (go reaction time) in both the practice phase and the main task, and changes in post-signal adjustment. These variables did not indicate any significant differences between problem gamblers and healthy controls.

SSRT is considered the primary index of response inhibition on this task. Our finding of slower SSRTs in alcohol dependence extends previous reports in other forms of substance addiction including amphetamine (Monterosso et al. 2005) and cocaine (Fillmore and Rush 2002; Li et al. 2006) abuse. However, in these studies of current or abstinent substance users, it is not possible to separate whether a neurocognitive effect pre-dates the addiction, or arises as a consequence of prolonged substance use. In our study, SSRT did not differ in problem gamblers and healthy controls (mean = 185 ms and 184 ms, respectively, compared with 212 ms in the alcohol-dependent group). Problem gambling is argued to represent a behavioural addiction that shares underlying vulnerability mechanisms with substance addiction (e.g. Potenza 2006), but without the damaging effects of drug use on brain structure and/or function. By this reasoning, the intact stop-signal performance in the problem gamblers may indicate that the deficit in alcohol dependence arises as a consequence of long-term alcohol consumption. Functional neuroimaging studies of the stop-signal test implicate a distributed cortical and subcortical network supporting efficient response inhibition, with a key foci in the lateral prefrontal cortex (PFC; more specifically, the right inferior frontal gyrus; Aron et al. 2003; Chambers et al. 2006). This is a likely substrate for the progressive effect in alcohol dependence; functional imaging studies have shown impaired recruitment of lateral prefrontal cortex during tasks of executive control in alcohol dependence (Dao-Castellana et al. 1998; Pfefferbaum et al. 2001; Schecklmann et al. 2007), neuropsychological studies show acute effects of alcohol and binge drinking upon executive functions associated with this region (Weissenborn and Duka 2003), and structural imaging studies indicate frontal lobe shrinkage as a function of increased alcohol use (Kubota et al. 2001). By inference, the deficits in response inhibition observed in previous studies of stimulant and Ecstasy users (e.g. Clark et al. 2006; Fillmore and Rush 2002; Quednow et al. 2007) may also arise as a progressive consequence of drug exposure on frontal lobe function.

Reduced dorsolateral PFC activity was reported in a recent functional imaging study of the stop-signal test in alcohol-dependent individuals, in the key contrast of successful inhibitions minus inhibition failures (Li et al. 2009). The behavioural data in that study did not reveal any difference in SSRT between the alcohol-dependent group (mean = 190 ms) and healthy controls (mean = 195 ms), but it should be noted that subjects were practiced extensively on the task (for 40 min) prior to scanning, and this is likely to have severely blunted the behavioural sensitivity of the test. Our own findings are consistent with three recent neuropsychological studies that used the stop-signal test in alcohol dependence; Goudriaan et al. (2006) reported slower SSRTs compared with controls, but did not report other performance measures on the task. Glass et al. (2009) reported associations between both SSRT and go reaction time and measures of alcoholism severity in a large mixed sample of alcohol-dependent and non-alcohol-dependent men (n = 240). Wojnar et al. (2009) reported SSRTs close to our own in alcohol-dependent patients with (mean = 230 ms) and without (mean = 216 ms) a history of suicide attempt, but did not include a healthy control group for comparison, and also did not report other performance measures from the test.

The previous studies of stop-signal performance in stimulant users reported single dissociations between impaired SSRT and intact performance on basic Go responding (Fillmore and Rush 2002; Li et al. 2006; Monterosso et al. 2005). In our data, the alcohol-dependent group was additionally slowed in go reaction time. This is unlikely to represent a strategic decision to delay the response (i.e. in order to increase the likelihood of stop-signal detection), as the response latency was already slowed during a practice block prior to introduction of the stop signals, and did not change significantly between the practice and main task. Longer go reaction times were also seen in the functional imaging study of the stop-signal task in alcohol dependence (Li et al. 2009), and general psychomotor slowing is a common finding in alcohol-dependent samples (De Wilde et al. 2007). In our study, the slowing in go reaction time was associated with duration of alcohol abuse, which further supports the argument of a progressive effect of long-term alcohol use.

In addition, the alcohol-dependent group demonstrated impaired behavioural adjustment following failed inhibitions on the task. The healthy controls slowed their Go responding on trials following failed stops, compared with Go trials that followed other Go trials. This slowing of responses after an error is an adaptive process that has been associated with performance-monitoring and error-detection in medial prefrontal cortex (Kerns et al. 2004). This adjustment was abolished in the alcohol-dependent group, who were (non-significantly) faster following failed stops. Moreover, this change in response latency was related to an index of clinical severity, the SADQ score. Deficient post-error slowing on the stop-signal procedure has been reported in abstinent cocaine users (Li et al. 2006) and may be related to hypofunction in medial or dorsolateral PFC during error detection (Kaufman et al. 2003; Li et al. 2009). Acute dosing of alcohol also disrupted error-related brain activity in healthy volunteers (Ridderinkhof et al. 2002). Thus, our findings corroborate other recent indications of abnormal behavioural adjustment processes in addiction.

The stop-signal test was used in two previous studies of problem gamblers. In the study by Rodriguez-Jimenez et al. (2006), there was no observable difference in SSRT between pathological gamblers and controls, but the authors report that those gamblers with ADHD features were significantly impaired in comparison to gamblers without ADHD features (neither group differed significantly from controls). However, Goudriaan et al. (2006) did report a significant difference in SSRT between treatment-seeking pathological gamblers and controls, which was also seen in two further groups with alcohol dependence and Tourette syndrome. Unfortunately, they did not report other task variables besides SSRT. The literature on broader aspects of executive function in problem gambling is also inconsistent (Cavedini et al. 2002; Forbush et al. 2008; Kalechstein et al. 2007; Leiserson and Pihl 2007), and it is conceivable that lateral PFC pathophysiology may give rise to a dysexecutive-type syndrome in only a subset of the most severe pathological gamblers (for example, the antisocial impulsive gamblers described by Blaszczynski and Nower (2002)). Whilst our gamblers were a community-recruited sample rather than a clinical group (due to the lack of available treatment facilities in the UK), the mean SOGS score of 9.8 is within the range of previous research (mean SOGS 9–13). Our group were also impaired on two tests of risky decision-making, described separately (Lawrence et al. 2009), and thus, it is unlikely that we simply recruited an anomalous high-functioning sample. It is also notable that we confirmed an elevation in trait impulsivity in our group, consistent with many previous studies (Verdejo-Garcia et al. 2008 for review). In this respect, our finding of elevated trait impulsivity in problem gamblers coupled with no significant difference on the stop-signal test echoes a recent paper by Forbush et al. (2008), who found that personality measures including impulsivity added significant incremental variance over a neuropsychological assessment in discriminating pathological gamblers and controls, but that neuropsychological variables did not add significantly over personality measures. Measures of trait impulsivity have also been shown to predict treatment retention in problem gamblers (Leblond et al. 2003) and to mediate depressive symptoms in regular gamblers (Clarke 2006).

As a final point, our measure of compulsivity detected only modest differences across the three groups, in contrast to the clear effects on trait impulsivity. There was no overall difference in the Padua Inventory total score, and a significant effect on the checking subscale was driven predominantly by the alcohol-dependent group, with a non-significant trend increase in the problem gamblers. Problem gambling has been conceptualised by some as aligned with OCD on an ‘impulsive-compulsive spectrum’ (Grant and Potenza 2006; Hollander and Wong 1995; McElroy et al. 1994), and some psychometric studies have reported increased compulsivity ratings in problem gamblers (Blaszczynski 1999; Frost et al. 2001). However, other data have failed to find significant co-morbidity between the two disorders (Kessler et al. 1994; Potenza 2006). We would support the recent conclusion by Blanco et al. (2009) that whilst both impulsive and compulsive features may be detectable in problem gamblers, the impulsive features predominate and are more closely associated with gambling severity.

In conclusion, this study has characterised performance on a neurocognitive probe of response inhibition, the stop-signal test, in alcohol dependence and problem gambling. The alcohol-dependent group were impaired on multiple aspects of the task, including the core index of response inhibition (SSRT) but also measures of psychomotor speed and behavioural adjustment. The observed changes in SSRT and behavioural adjustment (post-signal slowing) extend previous reports in stimulant users, indicating cross-substance impairments in cognitive control in addiction. However, the absence of these impairments in a group with problem gambling, a putative behavioural addiction, implies that the observed changes in alcohol dependence (and other substance addictions) may arise as a detrimental effect of chronic substance intake on frontal lobe function.