Introduction

The neuro-endocrine response to stress includes the activation of the sympathetic nervous system, resulting in increased heart rate, and release of cortisol from the adrenal glands. High levels of cortisol lead to negative feedback of the stress response in the brain, by binding to the low affinity glucocorticoid receptor (GR) (de Kloet et al. 2009). The mineralocorticoid receptor (MR) has a high affinity for cortisol and plays a role in the tonic inhibition of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis (de Kloet et al. 2009).

Responses to stress are influenced by genetic factors (reviewed by DeRijk 2009); the availability and efficiency of corticoid receptors is influenced by variation in the genes coding for them. Small variants, called single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) are commonly present in the GR gene (nuclear receptor subfamily 3, group C, member 1 [NR3C1], OMIM +138040) and the MR gene (nuclear receptor subfamily 3, group C, member 2 [NR3C2], OMIM *600983). SNPs in both genes have been associated with cortisol and heart rate responses to stress (DeRijk 2009) and may convey a risk for stress-related disorders such as depression. This is supported by associations between GR SNPs and major depression (van Rossum et al. 2006; van West et al. 2006) and with depressive symptoms (Kuningas et al. 2006).

From animal studies we know that synthetic and natural sex hormones influence the response to stress by interacting with both GRs and MRs in the hypothalamus and amygdala (Turner 1997; Chrousos et al. 1998; Levine 2002). These interactions might be different according to genetically based functional differences in cortisol receptors. The results of these type of association studies add to the knowledge if and how genetically based variances in corticoid receptors influence HPA axis and cardiac autonomic responses to social stress. Moreover, gender-specific effects of genes have been associated with vulnerability to stress and depression (Silberg et al. 1999; Kendler et al. 2006), and the underlying mechanisms might involve interactions of female sex hormones with corticoid receptors.

The ER22/23EK (rs6189/rs6190) SNPs (G to A) in exon 2 of the GR gene (Russcher et al. 2005) was associated with corticoid resistance in a pharmacological challenge (Van Rossum et al. 2004), resulting in higher post-dexamethasone cortisol levels. In men, the G allele of the BclI SNP in the GR gene was associated with blunted salivary cortisol responses compared to men homozygous for the C allele (Wüst et al. 2004; Kumsta et al. 2007; Ising et al. 2008). Inverse relations were detected in women [all oral contraceptives (OC) users]; women homozygous for the G allele displayed higher cortisol responses than C-carriers (Kumsta et al. 2007). Contrary to Kumsta and colleagues, no gender difference was found by Ising et al. (2008); BclI G allele carriers of both genders displayed lower responses. The 9beta polymorphism in the GR gene is a A-to-G SNP present in the 3′ untranslated region of exon 9ß. Both men and women who carried the G allele displayed increased cortisol responses (Kumsta et al. 2007). The MRI180V (rs5522) polymorphism consists of a SNP in exon 2, resulting in an amino acid change in the N-terminal domain of the protein. In vitro, this SNP decreased the transactivation capacity of the protein (DeRijk et al. 2006). Additionally, male carriers of the C allele displayed enhanced cortisol and heart rate responses to a social stress test (DeRijk et al. 2006). Another G/C SNP, situated two nucleotides before the translation start site of the MR gene (-2G/C, rs2070951rs) was examined in vitro. The C allele was found to be associated with increased activity of the MR receptor (van Leeuwen et al. 2007). This SNP has not been examined in the context of social stress but was associated with the cortisol awakening response (CAR) after the dexamethasone suppression test in a gender-specific way (Van Leeuwen et al. 2010).

All previous associations were tested in adult samples and included mostly men. If women were included in the samples, they all used OC which are known to influence cortisol measurements (Kirschbaum et al. 1999; Rohleder et al. 2003, but not Brody 2002), which may have implications for the interpretation of previous reported associations in women. The underlying mechanism most likely involves the higher production of cortisol binding globuline (GBC) when estrogen levels are high (Wiegratz et al. 2003). CBG binds free cortisol resulting in less cortisol available to be detected in saliva. Indeed differences have been found according to gender and OC use in adolescence (Bouma et al. 2009). Although studies in adult women reported lower cortisol levels in the follicular phase (when estrogen levels are high) than in the luteal phase (Altemus et al. 1997; Kirschbaum et al. 1999; Wolf et al. 2001; Rohleder et al. 2003), this was not found in adolescent girls (Bouma et al. 2009).

In the present study we investigate if the aforementioned findings in adults could be replicated in adolescent boys and girls. We are the first to include girls using OC as well as free-cycling (FC) girls in the same study. The influence of the -2G/C SNP in the MR gene is tested for the first time in the context of a social stress situation. In order to compare our results with those of previous findings we analyzed the associations of the SNPs with the physiological responses for boys, FC girls and OC users separately. Since several associations will be tested a correction for the level of significance has to be applied (Sainani 2009).

Materials and methods

Sample description

Data were collected in a subsample of the TRacking Adolescents’ Individual Lives Survey (TRAILS). TRAILS is prospective cohort study of Dutch (pre)adolescents, with the aim to chart and explain the development of mental health from preadolescence into adulthood, both at the level of psychopathology and the levels of underlying vulnerability and environmental risk. The first wave (T1) ran from March 2001 to July 2002, the second wave (T2) from September 2003 to December 2004, and the third wave (T3) from September 2005 to December 2007. At T1, 2,230 children were enrolled in the study (mean age 11.09, SD = 0.59), response rate 76%, de Winter et al. 2005). At T2, 96.4% of the baseline sample participated again (mean age 13.55, SD = 0.54), and at T3 the response rate was 81.4% (mean age 16.13, SD = 0.59). Currently data collection for the fourth wave is being conducted, which will be completed by the end of 2010. For a more detailed description of the TRAILS cohort see Huisman et al. (2008).

At the third assessment wave (T3) a focus sample was selected from the total TRAILS sample (n = 1816) and invited to participate in a series of laboratory tasks in order to study a diversity of research questions within the TRAILS study. Of the 744 adolescents that were invited 715 (96.1%) agreed to participate. This sample consisted of 473 (66.2%) adolescents with an increased risk to develop mental health problems in order to increase the power to detect mental health-related differences in response patterns. Increased risk was defined based on baseline temperament (high scores on frustration and fearfulness, low scores on effortful control), parental psychopathology (depression, anxiety, addiction, psychoses, or antisocial behavior), and environmental risk (living in a single-parent family). The other 33.8% (n = 242) were randomly selected from the total TRAILS sample. Please note that, although increased risk adolescents were oversampled, the sample included the total range of mental health problems present in a community population of adolescents, only in a different distribution. The experimental protocol was approved by the Dutch Central Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects (CCMO).

Within the focus sample, 649 (90.8%) adolescents were of Caucasian descent, of whom 592 (91.2%) were successfully genotyped. We excluded adolescents who used analgesic (pain-relieving) drugs on the day of the experiment or the day before and/or SSRIs and/or corticosteroids (n = 24), because these medications can influence responses to stress (Holsboer and Barden 1996). Three girls were discarded because of missing information on OC-use. Eight adolescents were discarded because of detection failures in more than two saliva samples and/or irregularities in two or more interbeat-intervals (IBI) recording blocks. Hence, 553 adolescents (mean age 16.07 years, SD = 0.90, FC-girls n = 183; OC-girls n = 93; boys n = 277) were included in the analyses.

Procedures

During the experimental session, participants’ psychophysiological responses (cardiovascular, cortisol, subjective arousal) to a variety of challenging conditions were recorded (see Bouma et al. 2009 for details). In brief, the sessions took place on weekdays, in soundproof rooms with blinded windows at selected locations in the participants’ residence town; lasted about 3 h; and started between 0800 and 0930 h or between 1230 and 0230 h (57%). Adolescents were randomly assigned to morning and afternoon sessions. The ratio of boys, FC-girls and OC-girls was not different between the morning and the afternoon sessions (χ2 (557,2) = 0.35, p = 0.84. Participants were asked to refrain from smoking and from using coffee, milk, chocolate, and other sugar-containing foods in the 2 h before the session. Data of morning and afternoon sessions were pooled.

The Groningen Social Stress Test (GSST)

The GSST is a standardized protocol, inspired by the Trier Social Stress Task (TSST; Kirschbaum et al. 1993), to induce moderate performance-related acute social stress. In short, participants were instructed to prepare a 6-min speech about themselves and their lives and perform difficult mental arithmetic in front of a camera. The GSST encompasses the three most important triggers of HPA axis: uncontrollability, threat of failure, and fear of negative social evaluation (Dickerson and Kemeny 2002). Participants were debriefed directly after the GSST. For a more detailed description of the GSST, please see Bouma et al. (2009).

Genotyping

DNA was extracted from blood samples or (in a few cases) buccal swabs (Cytobrush®) using a manual salting out procedure as described by Miller et al. (1988). SNPs were selected on the basis of known or expected functionality and/or previous reported associations with physiological responses to stress. GR N363S (rs6195) was included in the initial selection but could not be genotyped because of incompatibility with other SNPs on the Illumina platform. Genotyping was performed on the Golden Gate Illumina BeadStation 500 platform (Illumina Inc., San Diego, CA, USA), following the manufacturers protocol. We used an assay which was designed within the framework of various research questions of the TRAILS study. Genotyping data and clustering was performed in BeadStudio 3.0 (Illumina Inc., San Diego, CA, USA). Call rates were 95% for rs6198 (GR 9beta) and rs5522 (MR I180V), and 100% for rs2070951 (MR-2G/C) and rs41423247 (GR BclI). Call rate of GR ER22 (rs6189) was lower (81%) than the threshold of 95% and was excluded from statistical analysis. Concordance between DNA replicates showed a genotyping accuracy of 100%. Data cleaning was in line with procedures recommended by Nolte et al. (2010). Calculations of linkage disequilibrium between SNPs were performed in Excel using an Expectation-Maximization algorithm.

Measures

Cortisol was assessed from saliva by the Salivette sampling device (Sarstedt, Numbrecht, Germany). The cortisol response to social stress was measured based on four cortisol samples. Sample 1 was taken just before the start of the GSST and reflects HPA axis activity when participants filled out a rating scale while sitting quietly. Sample 2 was collected directly after the end of the GSST and reflects HPA axis responses during the start of the GSST. Sample 3 reflects HPA axis activity around the end of the GSST. Sample 4 was collected 40 min after the end of the GSST and is considered to reflect post-stress activity of the HPA axis. Samples were brought to the laboratory of the University Medical Center in Groningen where they were stored at −20°C until analysis. The intra-assay coefficient of variation was 8.2% for concentrations of 1.5 nmol/l, 4.1% for concentrations of 15 nmol/l, and 5.4% for concentrations of 30 nmol/l. The inter-assay coefficients of variation were, respectively, 12.6, 5.6, and 6.0%. The detection border was 0.9 nmol/l. Missing cortisol samples (C1, n = 20, C2, n = 24, C3, n = 27, C4, n = 24) were due to detection failures in the lab (52%) or insufficient saliva in the tubes (48%). Missing values were imputed on the basis of group mean and standard deviation, where group is either ‘Boys’, ‘FC-girls’, or ‘OC-girls’, and the (standardized) mean of participant’s non-missing cortisol samples.

Heart rate (HR) was recorded during and after the GSST in four blocks for several seconds: speech preparation (H1) (240 s), speech (H2) (360 s), mental arithmetic (H3) (360 s), and post test (H4) (300 s). A three-lead electrocardiogram was registered using 3M/RedDot–Ag/AgCl electrodes (type 2255, 3M Health Care, D-41453 Neuss, Germany), while the participant was sitting and breathing spontaneously. With a BIOPAC Amplifier-System (MP100), the signals were amplified and filtered before digitization at 250 samples/s. Dedicated software (PreCARSPAN), previously used in, e.g., Dietrich et al. (2006) was used to check signal stationarity, to correct for artifacts, to detect R-peaks, and to calculate the IBI between two heart beats. IBI is inversely related to heart rate by the equation HR = 60000/IBI. HR was defined as the number of beats per minute (bpm). Blocks were considered invalid if they contained artifacts with duration of more than 5 s, if the total artifact duration was more than 10% of the registration, or if the block length was less than 100 s. Missing heart rate recordings (H1, n = 6, H2, n = 3, H3, n = 3, H4, n = 6) were due to recording failure (42%) or signal-analysis failure (58%). Missing values were imputed in a similar way as described above for cortisol levels.

Cortisol and heart rate response variables were computed to analyze if the GSST induced significant increases in cortisol and heart rate. We determined the peak response (indicated by either C2 or C3 for cortisol and H2 or H3 for heart rate) and subtracted the pretest measure (C1 or H1) from this peak. A difference larger than zero was considered a meaningful increase.

Use of OC was assessed at the day of the experiment, while type and name of the pill were asked as part of a questionnaire that was assessed previously, at school. In total, OCs were used by 93 girls, of whom 66 (70.2%) used a monophasic OC and four girls a tri-phasic Pill (both ethinylestradiol and progestagen based). Ten girls used the ‘Diane’ Pill, which contains only progestagen (cyproteronacetaat) and is mainly prescribed for the beneficial effects on acne vulgaris. Thirteen girls did not know what type of OC they used.

Depressed mood was assessed at the start of the experimental session, by means of the Dutch version of the short Profile of Mood Scale (POMS; Wald and Mellenbergh 1990). The Depressed Mood scale includes eight items describing current mood (down, helpless, sad, lonely, unhappy, unworthy, melancholic, desperate), which could be rated on a five-point scale (1 = not at all, 2 = a little, 3 = partly, 4 = kind of, 5 = very much). Cronbach’s alpha was 0.87.

Statistical analysis

Analyses were performed in SPSS (Version 14.0). Allele frequencies were calculated and analyzed for deviations from Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (HWE) using χ2-tests with 1 degree of freedom. Means and standard deviations of all variables were calculated and gender differences were analyzed by Analysis of Variance (continuous variables) or χ2-tests (categorical variables). Cortisol and heart rate data were log-transformed (natural logarithm) to approach a normal distribution before analysis. Tables and figures show non-transformed data. Genotypes were examined concordant with earlier studies; GR 23EK: AG versus GG; GR BclI: CC versus CG versus GG, GR 9beta and MR I180V: AA versus AG/GG (Wüst et al. 2004; Kumsta et al. 2007; Ising et al. 2008). The novel MR-2G/C polymorphism was examined by the general genotype approach (GG versus GC versus CC) assuming no specific inheritance model.

Genotype associations with cortisol and heart rate responses were examined by repeated-measures General Linear Modeling (GLM), with cortisol (C1, C2, C3, C4) or heart rate (H1, H2, H3, H4) responses to the GSST as dependent variables, and genotype as between-subjects factor. When sphericity could not be assumed effects were corrected using the Greenhouse-Geisser procedure. Analyses were performed for each SNP separately. Interactions between genotype and cortisol or heart rate indicate genotypic differences in changes in cortisol or heart rate during the task. In case of significant interactions, linear and quadratic trends were examined to explore the nature of the differences. A linear trend signifies an overall rise or fall in cortisol levels or heart rate during the test, while a quadratic trend indicates higher levels during the test than before and after. Analyses were performed separately for boys, FC-girls and OC-girls. Depressed mood was included as a covariate since it may be associated with both the genotypes under study and cortisol and heart rate responses to stress (Hughes and Stoney 2000; Burke et al. 2005; van Rossum et al. 2006; van West et al. 2006; Kuningas et al. 2006; Carroll et al. 2007). Analyses were controlled for time of day (morning or afternoon). Menstrual cycle phase was not considered in the analyses. This was reasoned from the absence of an effect of cycle phase on the cortisol response in a previous study of this adolescent sample (Bouma et al. 2009).

Since 30 associations were tested (five SNP genotypes with two phenotypes in three 3 groups), a correction for multiple testing had to be applied. We used the false discovery rate (FDR) procedure proposed by Benjamini and Hochberg (1995). As the response phenotypes (in this case cortisol and heart rate) were correlated (r = 0.17, we applied the FDR correction for each phenotype separately, resulting in an alpha threshold of 0.003.

Results

Descriptive statistics

All SNPs were in HWE. Frequencies of alleles and genotypes can be found in Table 1. The two SNPs in the GR gene and in the MR gene were not correlated with each other (r 2 < 0.2 for both pairs). The linkage disequilibrium measure D′ was >0.98 for both pairs, indicating strongly reduced recombination between the SNPs within a gene, but independent evolutionary histories of the mutations. Genotype frequencies did not differ between boys, FC-girls, and OC-girls (shown in Table 1).

Table 1 Allele frequencies and differences between boys, FC girls and OC users

Table 2 shows descriptive statistics, separately for the three groups (Boys, FC-girls, and OC-girls). The group difference for age was due to the fact that OC-girls were somewhat older than FC-girls (p = 0.001). OC using girls had higher cortisol levels on sample 1 compared to FC-girls (p < 0.001) and boys (p = 0.001). Boys had higher cortisol levels on sample 2 (p < 0.01) compared to FC-girls. Group differences in heart rate were caused during preparation and speech by boys having lower heart rates than girls (p < 0.001), regardless of OC use. The group differences during arithmetic was caused by boys having lower heart rate than FC-girls (p < 0.001) and the difference during post stress was due to boys having lower heart rates than OC-girls (p = 0.01). Girls, irrespective of OC-use, reported more depressed mood than boys (p < 0.02).

Table 2 Descriptive statistics

Cortisol and heart rate responses were associated in FC-girls (r = 0.27; p < 0.01) and in boys (r = 0.15; p < 0.001) but not in OC-girls. A rise in cortisol levels between the pretest (C1) and levels during the stress tests (C2 and C3) was observed in most FC-girls (72%) and boys (78%). As expected from previous findings in this sample (Bouma et al. 2009), a rise in cortisol levels was largely absent in OC-girls; only 40% showed a small increase. Heart rate increased from pretest (H1) to speech (H2) or mental arithmetic (H3) in most participants; FC-girls (81%), boys (80%), and OC-girls (72%).

No associations between SNP genotype and physiological stress responses

The results of the tests for interaction effects of genotype with cortisol and heart rate can be found in Tables 3 (cortisol) and 4 (heart rate), for boys, FC-girls and OC-girls separately indicated. When applying our adjusted alpha threshold of 0.003, to account for the consequences of multiple testing, no significant associations are present.

Table 3 Effects of SNP genotype on the cortisol response to the GSST
Table 4 Effects of SNP genotype on the heart rate response to the GSST

Discussion

Our aim was to replicate previous reports of associations between SNPs in the GR and MR genes and HPA axis and cardiac autonomic responses to social stress in a large sample of adolescents. We examined associations separately for boys, FC-girls and OC-girls. None of the previously reported associations in adult samples could be replicated in this large adolescent sample. GR9beta was not associated with cortisol in boys and OC-girls in our adolescent sample as reported previously by Kumsta et al. (2007). Findings regarding GRBclI and cortisol in men and OC-girls (Wüst et al. 2004; Kumsta et al. 2007; Ising et al. 2008) were also not replicated. The association between MR I180V and heart rate response patterns previously reported in adults (DeRijk et al. 2006) was also not present in adolescents. None of the GR variants was associated with heart rate which partly supports the suggestion by DeRijk and de Kloet (2008) that cardiac autonomic responses do not seem to be affected by GR variants. The MR-2G/C SNP was also not associated with cortisol and heart rate in any of the three groups.

Inconsistencies in results between the present and previous studies could be due to differences in sample characteristics and dissimilarity in the application of correction for multiple testing. First, whereas previous samples consisted of (young) adults, our sample concerned adolescents. The adolescent brain seems especially sensitive to psychosocial stress (Perlman et al. 2007). Genetically based differences in corticoid receptors could affect the activation and feedback of the HPA axis during stressful situations differently in adolescence compared to adulthood. Second, long-term use of synthetic sex hormones might have other effects on physiological stress responses than the relatively short use (max. 18 months) in our adolescent sample. Longitudinal research, such as the TRAILS study, is needed to elucidate the long-term effects of OC use on HPA axis functioning (Ott et al. 2008). Third, the question might be raised if oversampling of increased-risk adolescents affected the distribution of genotypes in our study. This is not the case; allele frequencies of the SNPs under study did not differ between the total number of genotyped participants in the TRAILS focus sample (n = 592) and the TRAILS population sample (n = 1246) (data available on request). In addition to this, the cortisol response and heart rate response was not different according to risk status (increased or low) (results of these analyses also available on request). Fourth, the cortisol levels reflecting HPA axis activation during speech and arithmetic were lower than those reported in adults. This might be due to age-related differences as previously discussed, but may also be explained by differences in the stress protocols used. The main difference between the TSST and the GSST concerns the amount of social evaluation: a panel of three judges (TSST) versus one experimenter and a video camera (GSST). But even if this difference led to a lower stress appraisal compared to the TSST, we do not consider this a limitation of our study. Individual differences in stress responses to mild stressors may be just as important to investigate as the responses to more extreme stressors. Our GSST protocol induced significant cortisol responses, as well as meaningful between-group differences. Fifth, another reason for discrepancies is the fact that the results of the present study were corrected for multiple testing while previous results were not (except for those reported by Ising et al. 2008). Initially, we found four significant associations, three in the GR and one in the MR gene, but after correcting the alpha threshold for multiple testing, these results could not be considered as statistically valid. It should be noted that the associations we found without correction for multiple testing were not in the same SNPs as reported before.

Our findings should be viewed in the light of the following limitations. First, the social stress test was the last task of the experimental session and we do not know to what extent this may have influenced heart rate and cortisol responses. However, systematic bias in our associations is not likely, because the social stress test was by far the most stressful element of the session (data available on request). Second, we examined physiological responses in morning and afternoon sessions, which could have made direct comparisons with the previous studies having afternoon sessions only, difficult. However, time of day does not influence cortisol response pattern, not in adults (Kudielka et al. 2004) and not in adolescent boys and FC girls (Bouma et al. 2009). Therefore, we see no reason to believe that this methodological difference between studies could have influenced the non-replication of previous results. Third, the social stress task involved speaking aloud, which may have obscured associations with genotype since Sloan et al. (1991) reported that speaking during a stressful task decreases heart rate responses. Fourth, correcting for depressed mood in the analyses might have mask effects as GR SNPs are associated with depression and depressed mood (van Rossum et al. 2006; van West et al. 2006; Kuningas et al. 2006). However, re-analyzing the data without including depressed mood as covariate did not change the results.

Since our sample size is much larger (>500) compared to those of previous studies (Wüst et al. 2004; DeRijk et al. 2006; Kumsta et al. 2007; Ising et al. 2008), and we had sufficient power (>80%) to detect a medium size effect (based on calculations recommended by Nolte et al. 2010), the question must be raised if some of the previously reported associations should be considered false positive findings.

Funding

TRAILS has been financially supported by various grants from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research NWO (Medical Research Council program grant GB-MW 940-38-011; ZonMW Brainpower grant 100-001-004; ZonMw Risk Behavior and Dependence grants 60-60600-98-018 and 60-60600-97-118; ZonMw Culture and Health grant 261-98-710; Social Sciences Council medium-sized investment grants GB-MaGW 480-01-006 and GB-MaGW 480-07-001; Social Sciences Council project grants GB-MaGW 457-03-018, GB-MaGW 452-04-314, and GB-MaGW 452-06-004; NWO large-sized investment grant 175.010.2003.005); the Sophia Foundation for Medical Research (projects 301 and 393), the Dutch Ministry of Justice (WODC), the European Science Foundation (EuroSTRESS project FP-006), and the participating universities. These sponsors had no involvement in study design, collection, analysis and interpretation of the data, nor in writing or decisions regarding the submission of the paper.