Skip to main content
Log in

Visual Fixation Patterns during Reciprocal Social Interaction Distinguish a Subgroup of 6-Month-Old Infants At-Risk for Autism from Comparison Infants

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Thirty-one infant siblings of children with autism and 24 comparison infants were tested at 6 months of age during social interaction with a caregiver, using a modified Still Face paradigm conducted via a closed-circuit TV-video system. In the Still Face paradigm, the mother interacts with the infant, then freezes and displays a neutral, expressionless face, then resumes interaction. Eye tracking data on infant visual fixation patterns were recorded during the three episodes of the experiment. Using a hierarchical cluster analysis, we identified a subgroup of infants demonstrating diminished gaze to the mother’s eyes relative to her mouth during the Still Face episode. Ten out of the 11 infants in this subgroup had an older sibling with autism.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We chose to use only the eyes and mouth in the denominator (and not the nose, or other less fixated regions such as the hair, forehead, and chin) for two reasons: (a) the unusual visual fixation pattern among individuals with autism reported by Klin et al. (2002) concerned these two regions, which are important for social communication, and (b) gaze to other face areas showed less variability between groups, and across episodes, than the eyes and mouth; including these areas was, in effect, to include a noisy term.

  2. The cluster analysis assigns infants to clusters using a proximity matrix of the distance of each subject’s score from every other subject’s score (the distance between subjects is calculated using the sum of squared Euclidean distances for each variable). Ward’s agglomeration method operates on this resulting matrix using an algorithm that iteratively fuses cases (or lower-order clusters of cases) into higher-order clusters with the criterion that the resulting error sum of squares is minimized. In order to ensure that input order did not influence the clusters that were generated, we used a randomization procedure to run 1,000 different solutions (see van der Kloot, Spaans, & Heiser, 2005). Results showed that the cluster solution remained identical throughout 1,000 trials, with relatively good fit (SSDIF = 2.47E + 007; cophenetic correlation = 0.66).

  3. The number of clusters was determined by comparing the relative amount of between-cluster variance in EMI scores accounted for by 2-, 3-, and 4-cluster models. For 2 clusters, 18.27% of the total variance was accounted for by cluster; for 3 clusters, 29.49%; and for 4 clusters, 21.73%. Therefore, the three-cluster model was judged to account for the maximum amount of between-cluster variance, while maintaining parsimony regarding the number of clusters. The three-cluster model provided a good fit to the observed data: of the 33 infants in the high EMI cluster, 25 had EMI scores above 0.5 during all three episodes, and 6 had EMI scores above 0.5 during the Still- and post-Still Face episodes. Nine out of 11 infants in the low EMI cluster had scores below 0.5 for all three episodes. All of the 11 low–high–low infants had EMI scores below 0.5 during the pre-Still Face, above 0.5 during the Still Face, and below 0.5 during the post-Still Face.

References

  • Aldenderfer, M. S., & Blashfield, R. K. (1984). Cluster analysis (Sage University Paper series on Quantative Applications in the Social Sciences, No. 07-044). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

  • American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th edn., text revision (DSM-IV-TR). Washington DC.

  • Bailey, A., Palferman, S., Heavey, L., & Le Couteur, A. (1998). Autism: The phenotype in relatives. J Autism Dev Disord, 28(5), 369–392.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Haim, Y., Shulman, C., Lamy, D., & Reuveni, A. (2006). Attention to eyes and mouth in high-functioning children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 131–137.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berument, S. K., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Pickles, A., & Bailey, A. (1999). Autism screening questionnaire: Diagnostic validity. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 444–451.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bolton, P., Macdonald, H., Pickles, A., Rios, P., Goode, S., & Crowson, M., et al. (1994). A case-control family history study of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35(5), 877–900.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G. (2003). Teleological and referential understanding of action in infancy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 447–458.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H. S., Gernsbacher, M. A., & Goldsmith, H. H., et al. (2005). Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience, 8(4), 519–526.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., & Bookheimer, S. Y., et al. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9(1), 28–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davies, S., Bishop, D., Manstead, A. S. R., & Tantam, D. (1994). Face perception in children with autism and Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35(6), 1033–1057.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Carver, L., Meltzoff, A. N., Panagiotides, H., McPartland, J., & Webb, S. J. (2002). Neural correlates of face and object recognition in young children with autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and typical development. Child Development, 73(3), 700–717.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Osterling, J., Meltzoff, A. N., & Kuhl, P. (2000). Case study of the development of an infant with autism from birth to two years of age. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21(3), 299–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., & McPartland, J. (2005). Understanding the nature of face processing impairment in autism: Insights from behavioral and electrophysiological studies. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(3), 403–424.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., Wijsman, E., Schellenberg, G., Estes, A., & Munson, J., et al. (2005). Neurocognitive and electrophysiological evidence of altered face processing in parents of children with autism: Implications for a model of abnormal development of social brain circuitry in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 17(3), 679–697.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dorris, L., Espie, C. A. E., Knott, F., & Salt, J. (2004). Mind-reading difficulties in the siblings of people with Asperger’s syndrome: Evidence for a genetic influence in the abnormal development of a specific cognitive domain. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(2), 412–418.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dumont-Mathieu, T., & Fein, D. (2005). Screening for autism in young children: The Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT) and other measures. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 11(3), 253–262.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gusella, J. L., Muir, D., & Tronick, E. A. (1988). The effect of manipulating maternal behavior during an interaction on three- and six-month-olds’ affect and attention. Child Development, 59(4), 1111–1124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haith, M. M., Bergman, T., & Moore, M. J. (1977). Eye contact and face scanning in early infancy. Science, 198(4319), 853–855.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayhoe, M., & Ballard, D. (2005). Eye movements in natural behavior. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), 188–194.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988). What’s in a face? The case of autism. British Journal of Psychology, 79(4), 441–453.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hong, L. E., Mitchell, B. D., Avila, M. T., Adami, H., McMahon, R. P., & Thaker, G. K. (2006). Familial Aggregation of eye-tracking endophenotypes in families of schizophrenic patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(3), 259–264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hunnius, S., & Geuze, R. H. (2004). Developmental changes in visual scanning of dynamic faces and abstract stimuli in infants: A longitudinal study. Infancy, 6(2), 231–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jemel, B., Mottron, L., & Dawson, M. (2006). Impaired face processing in autism: Fact or artifact? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 1–16.

  • Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., & Morton, J. (1991). Newborns’ preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition, 40(1–2), 1–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(9), 809–816.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lainhart, J. E., Ozonoff, S., Coon, H., Krasny, L., Dinh, E., & Nice, J., et al. (2002). Autism, regression, and the broader autism phenotype. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 113(3), 231–237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langdell, T. (1978). Recognition of faces: An approach to the study of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 19(3), 255–268.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lansing, C. R., & McConkie, G. W. (2003). Word identification and eye fixation locations in visual and visual-plus-auditory presentations of spoken sentences. Perception & Psychophysics, 65(4), 536–552.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maurer, D., & Barrera, M. E. (1981). Infants’ perception of natural and distorted arrangements of a schematic face. Child Development, 52(1), 196–202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maurer, D., & Salapatek, P. (1976). Developmental changes in the scanning of faces by young infants. Child Development, 47(2), 523–527.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osterling, J. A., Dawson, G., & Munson, J. A. (2002). Early recognition of 1-year-old infants with autism spectrum disorder versus mental retardation. Development and Psychopathology, 14(2), 239–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pascalis, O., Deschonen, S., Morton, J., Deruelle, C., & Fabregrenet, M. (1995). Mothers face recognition by neonates – a replication and an extension. Infant Behavior & Development, 18(1), 79–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pelphrey, K. A., Sasson, N. J., Reznick, J. S., Paul, G., Goldman, B. D., & Piven, J. (2002). Visual scanning of faces in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(4), 249–261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ritvo, E. R., Jorde, L. B., Mason-Brothers, A., Freeman, B. J., & et al. (1990). The UCLA-University of Utah epidemiologic survey of autism: Recurrence risk estimates and genetic counseling. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146(8), 1032–1036.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sasson, N. J. (2006). The development of face processing in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(3), 381–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, R. T. (2005). Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: The role of the amygdala and fusiform face area. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23(2–3), 125–141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, R. T., Grelotti, D. J., Klin, A., Kleinman, J., Van der Gaag, C., & Marois, R., et al. (2003). The role of the fusiform face area in social cognition: Implications for the pathobiology of autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 358(1430), 415–427.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., Dijamco, A., Gratier, M., & Rozga, A. (2004). Early detection of core deficits in autism. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 10(4), 221–233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sumi, S., Taniai, H., Miyachi, T., & Tanemura, M. (2006). Sibling risk of pervasive developmental disorder estimated by means of an epidemiologic survey in Nagoya, Japan. Journal of Human Genetics.

  • Tarabulsy, G. M., Provost, M. A., Deslandes, J., St-Laurent, D., Moss, E., & Lemelin, J.-P., et al. (2003). Individual differences in infant still-face response at 6 months. Infant Behavior & Development, 26(3), 421–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teunisse, J.-P., & de Gelder, B. (1994). Do autistics have a generalized face processing deficit? International Journal of Neuroscience, 77(1–2), 1–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Toda, S., & Fogel, A. (1993). Infant response to the still-face situation at 3 and 6 months. Developmental Psychology, 29(3), 532–538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tronick, E., Als, H., Adamson, L., Wise, S., & Brazelton, T. B. (1978). The infant’s response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 17(1), 1–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van der Kloot, W. A., Spaans, A. M. J., & Heiser, W. J. (2005). Instability of hierarchical cluster analysis due to input order of the data: The PermuCLUSTER solution. Psychological Methods, 10(4), 468–476.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Volkmar, F. R., Sparrow, S. S., Rende, R. D., & Cohen, D. J. (1989). Facial perception in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30(4), 591–598.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yarbus, A. L. (1961). Eye movements during the examination of complicated objects. Biofizika, 6(2), 52–56.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yirmiya, N., Gamliel, I., Pilowsky, T., Feldman, R., Baron-Cohen, S., & Sigman, M. (2006). The development of siblings of children with autism at 4 and 14 months: Social engagement, communication, and cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(5), 511–523.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S. E., Brian, J., Roberts, W., & Szatmari, P. (2005). Developmental spectrum among infant siblings of children with autism. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Atlanta, Georgia.

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) grant number MH068398 to S. Ozonoff, P.I., a National Association for Autism Research (NAAR) grant to S. Rogers, a Cure Autism Now (CAN) grant to S. Rogers, and Medical Investigation of Neurodevelopmental Disorders Institute (MIND Institute) grants to S. Rogers and to N. Merin. We wish to thank the families who took part, Mary Beth Steinfeld, Jeslin West, Joel Steele, and the contributions of undergraduate students Linh Dang, Joseph Marsano, Kawshalya Pathiraja, Alex Rajan, and Maryette Sabater. We are grateful to Scott Johnson, PhD., New York University, for sharing infant eye tracking methods with us, and to Darwin Muir, Ph.D., Queen’s University, and Daphne Maurer, PhD., McMaster University, for their input in the development and design of the experiment.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sally J. Rogers.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Merin, N., Young, G.S., Ozonoff, S. et al. Visual Fixation Patterns during Reciprocal Social Interaction Distinguish a Subgroup of 6-Month-Old Infants At-Risk for Autism from Comparison Infants. J Autism Dev Disord 37, 108–121 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0342-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0342-4

Keywords

Navigation