Skip to main content
Log in

Language Assessment and Development in Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorders

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

One of the primary diagnostic criteria for the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders (ASD) is the presence of a language delay or impairment. Children with ASD are now being identified at significantly younger ages, and prior research has consistently found that early language skills in this population are heterogeneous and an important predictor for later outcome. The goal of this study was to systematically investigate language in toddlers with ASD and to identify early correlates of receptive and expressive language in this population. The study included 164 toddlers with ASD between the ages of 18 and 33 months who were evaluated on several cognitive, language and behavioral measures. Results suggested good agreement among different measures of early language, including direct assessment and parent report measures. Significant concurrent predictors of receptive language included gestures, non-verbal cognitive ability and response to joint attention. For expressive language, the most significant predictors were non-verbal cognitive ability, gestures and imitation. These findings have important implications for intervention programs targeting this population.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (Fourth ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

  • Arbib, M. A., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Neural expectations: A possible evolutionary path from manual skills to language. Communication & Cognition, 29(3–4), 393–424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, A., Phillips, W., & Rutter, M. (1996). Autism: Towards an integration of clinical, genetic, neuropsychological, and neurobiological perspectives. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 89–126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, D. (1995). Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 131–158). Lawrence: Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1975). A comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 127–145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., & Dick, F. (2002). Language, gesture and the developing brain. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 293–310.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., Thal, D., Whitesell, K., Fenson, L., & Oakes, L. (1989). Integrating language and gesture in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 25(6), 1004–1019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M., Haynes, M., & Painter, K. (1998). Sources of child vocabulary competence: A multivariate model. Journal of Child Language, 25, 367–393.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bosseler, A., & Massaro, D. (2003). Development and evaluation of a computer-animated tutor or vocabulary and language learning in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 653–672.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boucher, J. (2003). Language development in autism. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinilaryngology, 67S1, S159–S163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J., & Sherwood, V. (1983). Thought, language and intersection in infancy. In J. D. Call, E. Galenson, & R. L. Tyson (Eds.), Frontiers of Infant Psychiatry (pp. 38–52). New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., Tomasello, M., Butterworth, G., & Moore, C. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(4), 1–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Pennington, B., & Rogers, S. (2002). Interrelations among social-cognitive skills in young children with autism. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders., 32(2), 91–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Tomasello, M., & Striano, T. (2005). Role reversal imitation and language in typically developing infants and children with autism. Infancy, 8(3), 253–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T. (2004). Matching preschool children with autism spectrum disorders and comparison children for language ability: Methodological challenges. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(1), 59–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T. (2006). Imitation and the development of language. In S. Rogers & J. Willams (Eds.), Imitation and the social mind: Autism and typical development (pp. 96–117). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2000). Testing joint attention, imitation and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15, 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2003). Predicting language outcome in infants with autism and pervasive developmental disorder. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38(3), 265–285.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Drew, A., Baird, C., & Baird, G. (2003). Measuring early language development in pre-school children with autism spectrum disorder using the MacArthur communicative development inventory (Infant Form). Journal of Child Language, 30, 213–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Taylor, E., Drew, A., Cockerill, H., Brown, J., & Baird, G. (2005). Outcome at 7 years of children diagnosed with autism at age 2: Predictive validity of assessments conducted at 2 and 3 years of age and pattern of symptom change over time. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 46(5), 500–513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

  • Condouris, K., Meyer, E., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The relationship between standardized measures of language and measures of spontaneous speech in children with autism. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12, 349–358.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., & Liaw, J. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 271–283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeGiacomo, A., & Fombonne, E. (1998). Parental recognition of developmental abnormalities in autism. European Journal of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 7, 131–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delgado, C., Mundy, P., Crowson, M., Markus, J., Yale, M., & Schwartz, H. (2002). Responding to joint attention and language development: A comparison of target locations. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 45, 715–719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Desrochers, S., Morissette, P., & Ricard, M. (1995). Two perspectives on pointing in infancy. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 85–101). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dyck, M. J., Piek, J. P., Hay, D., Smith, L., & Hallmayer, J. (2006). Are abilities abnormally interdependent in children with autism? Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 35(1), 20–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenmajer, R., Prior, M., Leekam, S., Wing, L., Ong, B., Gould, J., & Welham, M. (1998). Delayed language onset as a predictor of clinical symptoms in pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(6), 527–533.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J., Bates, E., Thal, D., & Pethick, S. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(5), 1–173.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J., Pethick, S., & Reilly, J. (1993). MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User's guide and technical manual. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J., Pethick, S., & Reilly, J. (2003). MacArthur communicative development inventories: User’s guide and technical manual. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Floel, A., Ellger, T., Breitenstein, C., & Knecht, S. (2003). Language perception activates the hand motor cortex: Implications for motor theories of speech perception. European Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 704–708.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ghaziuddin, M., Butler, E., Tsai, L., & Ghaziuddin, N. (1994). Is clumsiness a marker for Asperger syndrome? Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 38(5), 519–527.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S. (2000). Beyond words: The importance of gesture to researchers and learners. Child Development, 71(1), 231–239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16(3), 367–371.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kjelgaard, M. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16(2/3), 287–308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., & Smith, A. (1997). Variables related to differences in standardized test outcomes for children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 233–243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laakso, M.-L., Poikkeus, A.-M., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, P. (2000). Social interactional behaviors and symbolic play competence as predictors of language development and their associations with maternal attention-directing strategies. Infant Behavior & Development, 22(4), 541–556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “Theory of Mind”. Psychological Review, 94(4), 412–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., DiLavore, P., Shulman, C., Thurm, A., & Pickles, A. (2006). Autism from two to nine. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(6), 694–701.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P., Pickles, A., & Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule––Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., & Pickles, A. (2004). Trajectory of language development in autistic spectrum disorders. In M. Rice & S. Warren (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies (pp. 7–29). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., Goode, S., Heemsbergen, J., Jordan, H., Mawhood, L., & Schopler, E. (1989). Autism diagnostic observation schedule: A standardized observation of communicative and social behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 185–212.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). The autism diagnostic interview–Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Spence, S. J., Moldin, S. O., & Rubenstein, J. L. R. (2006). Autism spectrum disorders: Phenotype and diagnosis. In Understanding autism: From basic neuroscience to treatment (pp. 1–23). Boca Raton, FL, US: CRC Press.

  • Lord, C., & Ventner, A. (1992). Outcome and follow-up studies of high-functioning autistic individuals. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.), High-functioning individuals with autism (pp. 187–199). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loveland, K., & Landry, S. (1986). Joint attention and language in autism and developmental language delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16, 335–349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lyytinen, P., Ahonen, T., Eklund, K., Guttorm, T., Laakso, M.-L., Leinonen, S., Leppanen, P., Lyytinen, P., Poikkeus, A.-M., Puolakanaho, A., Richardson, U., & Viholainen, H. (2001). Developmental pathways of chidlren with and without familial risk for dyslexia during the first years of life. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(2), 535–554.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Masur, E. F., & Eichorst, D. L. (2002). Infants’ spontaneous imitation of novel versus familiar words: Relations to observational and maternal report measures of their lexicons. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48(4), 405–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDuffie, A., Yoder, P., & Stone, W. (2005). Prelinguistic predictors of vocabulary in young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(5), 1080–1097.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McEwen, F., Happé, F., Bolton, P., Rijsdijk, F., Ronald, A., Dworzynski, K., & Plomin, R. (2007). Origins of individual differences in imitation: Links with language, pretend play, and socially insightful behavior in two-year-old twins. Child Development, 78(2), 474–492.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meister, I. G., Boroojerdi, B., Foltys, H., Sparing, R., Huber, W., & Topper, R. (2003). Motor cortex hand area and speech: Implications for the development of language. Neuropsychologia, 41, 401–406.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morales, M., Mundy, P., Delgado, C., Yale, M., Neal, A. R., & Schwartz, H. (2000). Gaze following, temperament and language development in 6-month-olds: A replication and extension. Infant Behavior & Development, 23, 231–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, E. (1995). Mullen Scales of Early Learning. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Delgado, C., Block, J., Venezia, M., Hogan, A., & Seibert, J. (2003). A manual for the abridged early social communication scales (ESCS). Available through the university of Miami Psychology Department, Coral Gables, FL.

  • Mundy, P., & Hogan, A. (1996). Preliminary Manual for the Abridged Early Social Communication Scales. Coral Gables, FL: University of Miami.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 20(1), 115–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1994). Nonverbal communication, developmental level and symptom presentation in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 389–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1987). Nonverbal communication and play correlates of language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders., 17(3), 349–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, E. (2006). From imitation to conversation: The first dialogues with human neonates. Infant and Child Development, 15(3), 223–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noterdaeme, M., Mildenberger, K., Minow, F., & Amorosa, H. (2002). Evaluation of neuromotor deficits in children with autism and children with a specific speech and language disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 11, 219–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paul, R., & Fountain, R. (1999). Predicting outcomes of early expressive language delay. Infant-Toddler Intervention, 9(2), 123–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapin, I. (1996). Preschool children with inadequate communication. London: MacKeith Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Risi, S., Lord, C., Gotham, K., Corsello, C., Chrysler, C., Szatmari, P., Cook, E. H. Jr., Leventhal, B. L., & Pickles, A. (2006). Combining information from multiple sources in the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(9), 1094.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S., Charman, T., & Stone, W. (2006). Evidence-based interventions for language development in young children with autism. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S., Hepburn, S. L., Stackhouse, T., & Wehner, E. (2003). Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 44(5), 763–781.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seal, B., & Bonvillian, J. (1997). Sign language and motor functioning in students with autistic disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 437–466.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M. (1998). The Emanuel Miller memorial lecture 1997: Change and continuity in the development of children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 19(6), 817–827.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., & McGovern, C. (2005). Improvement in cognitive and language skills from preschool to adolescence in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(1), 15–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2002). The behaviors of parents of children with autism predict the subsequent development of their children’s communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(2), 77–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sparrow, S., Balla, D., & Cicchetti, D. (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stone, W., Ousley, O. Y., & Littleford, C. (1997). Motor imitation in young children with autism: What’s the object? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25(6), 475–485.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, W., & Yoder, P. J. (2001). Predicting spoken language level in children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 5(4), 341–361.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thorp, D., & Mundy, P. (submitted). Observational and parent report measures of social communication skills in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

  • Thurm, A., Lord, C., Lee, L., & Newschaffer, C. (2007). Predictors of language acquisition in preschool children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(9), 1731–1734.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Mervis, C. B. (1994). The instrument is great, but measuring comprehension is still a problem. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 174–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toth, K., Munson, J., Meltzoff, A. N., & Dawson, G. (2006). Early predictors of communication development in young children with autism spectrum disorder: Joint attention, imitation, and toy play. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(8), 993–1005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ungerer, J., & Sigman, M. (1984). The relation of play and sensorimotor behavior to language in the second year. Child Development, 55, 1448–1455.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Hecke, A. V., Mundy, P. C., Acra, C. F., Block, J. J., Delgado, C. E. F., Parlade, M. V., Meyer, J. A., Neal, A. R., & Pomares, Y. B. (2007). Infant joint attention, temperament, and social competence in preschool children. Child Development, 78(1), 53–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watt, N., Wetherby, A., & Shumway, S. (2006). Prelinguistic predictors of language outcome at 3 years of age. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(6), 1224–1237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Webster, R. I., & Shevell, M. I. (2004). Neurobiology of specific language impairment. Journal of Child Neurology, 19(7), 471–481.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, A., Woods, J., Allen, L., Cleary, J., Dickinson, H., & Lord, C. (2004). Early indicators of autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(5), 473–493.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Rogers, T., Roberts, W., Brian, J., & Szatmari, P. (2005). Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23, 143–152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by grants from NIH (U54 MH 66398; U19 DC 03610) and funding for the GCRC at Boston University School of Medicine (M01-RR0533). We thank the following people for their help in data collection and management: Anneliese Bass, Christine Connolly, Kristin Dame, Nicole Edwards, Amy Gower, Angelina Rayno and Gretchen Schuman. We also extend our sincere gratitude to the children and families who participated in this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Helen Tager-Flusberg.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Luyster, R.J., Kadlec, M.B., Carter, A. et al. Language Assessment and Development in Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 38, 1426–1438 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-007-0510-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-007-0510-1

Keywords

Navigation