Elsevier

Cognition

Volume 50, Issues 1–3, April–June 1994, Pages 363-384
Cognition

Some primitive mechanisms of spatial attention

https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)90036-1Get rights and content

Abstract

Our approach to studying the architecture of mind has been to look for certain extremely simple mechanisms which we have good reason to suspect must exist, and to confirm these empirically. We have been concerned primarily with certain low-level mechanisms in vision which allow the visual system to simultaneously index items at multiple spatial locations, and have developed a provisional model (called the FINST model) of these mechanisms. Among the studies we have carried out to support these ideas are ones showing that subjects can track multiple independent moving targets in a field of identical distractors, and that their ability to track these targets and detect changes occuring on them does not generalize to non-targets or to items lying inside the convex polygon that they form (so that a zoom lens of attention does not fit the data). We have used a visual search paradigm to show that (serial or parallel) search can be confined to a subset of indexed items and the layout of these items is of little importance. We have also carried out a large number of studies on the phenomenon known as subitizing and have shown that subitizing occurs only when items can be preattentively individuated and in those cases location precuing has little effect, compared with when counting occurs, which suggests that subitizing may be carried out by counting active indexes rather than items in the visual field. And finally we have run studies showing that a certain motion effect which is sensitive to attention can occur at multiple precued loci. We believe that taken as a whole the evidence is most parsimoniously accounted for in terms of the hypothesis that there is an early preattentive stage in vision where a small number of salient items in the visual field are indexed and thereby made readily accessible for a variety of visual tasks.

References (35)

  • C. Eriksen et al.

    Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model

    Perception and Psychophysics

    (1986)
  • B.D. Fisher et al.

    Multiple abrupt onset cues produce illusory line motion

    Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science

    (1993)
  • O. Hikosaka et al.

    Focal visual attention produces motion sensation in lines

    Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science

    (1991)
  • J. Intriligator et al.

    An object-specific spatial attentional facilitation that does not travel to adjacent spatial locations

    Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science

    (1992)
  • P. Jolicoeur

    Curve tracing operation and the perception of spatial relations

  • C. Koch et al.

    Shifts in selective visual attention: Towards the underlying neural circuitry

    Human Neurobiology

    (1985)
  • S.M. Kosslyn et al.

    On the demystification of mental imagery

    Behavioral and Brain Science

    (1979)
  • Cited by (118)

    • Fast automated counting procedures in addition problem solving: When are they used and why are they mistaken for retrieval?

      2016, Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Instead, the representations we hypothesize are closer to an object file in which each object is individuated (Feigenson et al., 2002; Simon, 1997). It is known that the use of object-file representations yields a set-size signature with success at representing sets up to four objects and failure with larger numbers (Kahneman, Treisman, & Gibbs, 1992; Pylyshyn, 1989; Pylyshyn, 1994). The involvement of this type of representation in very small problems would explain why their solving was affected by a strong size-effect, whereas RTs for small problems involving operands larger than four (the medium small problems) remained immune to size effect.

    • Neural and Behavioral Signatures of Core Numerical Abilities and Early Symbolic Number Development

      2015, Development of Mathematical Cognition: Neural Substrates and Genetic Influences: Volume 2
    • Two core systems of numerical representation in infants

      2014, Developmental Review
      Citation Excerpt :

      Recently, the OTS has been proposed to account for adults’ fast and accurate processing of small visual sets. The current OTS model (sometimes referred to as “parallel individuation”; Carey, 2004) is considered part of infants’ core knowledge of objects (Cheries, Mitroff, Wynn, & Scholl, 2009; Leslie, Xu, Tremoulet, & Scholl, 1998; Scholl & Leslie, 1999; Spelke & Kinzler, 2007) and is a reformulation of two previous models, the FINST mechanism (Pylyshyn, 1989, 1994, 2001; Pylyshyn & Storm, 1988; Trick & Pylyshyn, 1994) and the object file mechanism (Kahneman & Treisman, 1984; Kahneman, Treisman, & Gibbs, 1992), both of which are mechanisms of visual attention designed to explain humans’ ability to individuate and track visual objects and solve the problem of object correspondence. The OTS consists of a set of indexes that “point” to individual objects, allowing objects to be tracked through time and space (Kahneman et al., 1992; Pylyshyn, 1989).

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    The work reported herein was carried out under the author's direction at the Center for Cognitive Science, University of Western Ontario. Much of it has been reported in theses in the Department of Psychology and the Department of Electrical Engineering at the University of Western Ontario, by graduate students Brian Acton, Jacquie Burkell, Roy Eagleson, Paul McKeever, William Schmidt, Chris Sears and Lana Trick. These students, along with our Research Scientist Brian Fisher, contributed most of the empirical and theoretical work discussed in this paper. This research was supported by the Canadian Natural Science and Engineering Research Council (Grant A2600) and by a “Project B-4” grant from the Institute for Robotics and Intelligent Systems.

    Internet

    View full text