Prompting Methods affect the Accuracy of Children's School Lunch Recalls

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0002-8223(00)00264-9Get rights and content

Abstract

Objectives To evaluate the feasibility of implementing 3 specific prompting methods among students in the first and fourth grades (mean age=7.2 and 10.1 years, respectively), to validate recall accuracy of first- and fourth-grade students against observation, to develop a single measure of inaccuracy that cumulated errors in reporting food items and amounts without allowing underreporting and overreporting to cancel each other, and to establish information regarding the measure of inaccuracy for use in future studies.

Design Children were interviewed the morning after they were observed eating lunch provided by the school. Interviews included free recall, nonsuggestive prompted recall, and specific prompted recall (either preference, food category, or visual).

Subjects/setting Ninety-six children (48 students per grade) were studied—32 per the specific prompting method—stratified by grade, sex, and ethnicity (African-American or white). Specific prompting method was randomly assigned to children within strata.

Statistical analysis Food items were categorized and weighted as: combination entree=2, condiments=1/3, and other=1. To calculate inaccuracy of recall, absolute differences between amounts reported and observed eaten were calculated for each item and then multiplied by each item's weight; these values were then summed across all items for each child. Inaccuracy of recall was determined before specific prompting and after specific prompting and for the difference (inaccuracy after specific prompting minus inaccuracy before specific prompting).

Results Before specific prompting, median inaccuracy was 2.7 servings for the 48 first-grade students and 1.7 servings for the 48 fourth-grade students. The median difference in inaccuracy from before to after specific prompting was 0 for both grades. Specific prompting increased recall accuracy for 9 first-graders and 12 fourth-graders, but decreased recall accuracy for 21 first-graders and 7 fourth-graders, and did not change recall accuracy for 18 first-graders and 29 fourth-graders. Among children whose recall accuracy increased after specific prompting, 5 first-graders and 8 fourth-graders received prompting for food category.

Applications/conclusions Among first-grade students, specific prompting (either preference, food category, or visual) may hurt more than help recall accuracy. Among fourth-grade students, prompting for food category yields small gains in recall accuracy with minimal losses. Validation studies are needed with larger sample sizes to determine prompting methods that produce more accurate dietary recalls from children of various socioeconomic status groups, other racial/ethnic groups, and at other meals. J Am Diet Assoc. 2000;100:911-918.

Section snippets

Sample

The Human Assurance Committee at the Medical College of Georgia approved the study. Children were recruited from all 10 first-grade classes and all 8 fourth-grade classes from 2 elementary schools in Richmond County, Ga, during spring 1997. Children at the schools were of predominantly lower- to middle-socioeconomic status; 72% to 79% were eligible to receive free or reduced-price lunches during the data collection period. Approximately 68% of the children returned signed assent and parental

Length of Interviews

Interviews with first-graders ranged in length from 5 to 12 minutes with a mean and median of 8 minutes. With fourth-graders, interviews ranged in length from 4 to 11 minutes with a mean and median of 6 minutes. Interview length averaged 7, 8, and 9 minutes for the 16 first-graders in each of the preference, food category, and visual prompting groups, and 6,6, and 7 minutes for the 16 fourth-graders in each of the respective prompting groups.

Inaccuracy Before Specific Prompting and After Specific Prompting

The Table provides descriptive information regarding recall

Discussion

Dietary assessment methods typically include prompting to help people recall what they ate, but research validating the effects of prompting on the accuracy of dietary recalls against non-self–report methods is scarce. The first purpose of this study was achieved as children in both grades appeared responsive to different prompts.

The second purpose of this study was achieved as well. Before specific prompting, first-graders were more inaccurate than were fourth-graders. After specific

Applications

■ Among first-graders, specific prompting (either preference, food category, or visual) may hurt more than help recall accuracy. Among fourth-graders, food category prompting yields small gains in recall accuracy with minimal losses.

■ Validation studies are needed to determine which prompting methods produce the most accurate dietary recalls from children of various socioeconomic status groups, other racial/ethnic groups, and at other meals.

■ Dietetics practitioners need to know how to prompt

References (38)

  • G.C. Frank et al.

    Adapting the 24-hr recall for epidemiologic studies of school children

    J Am Diet Assoc.

    (1977)
  • S.D. Domel et al.

    How children remember what they have eaten

    J Am Diet Assoc.

    (1994)
  • S.B. Domel

    Self-reports of diethow children remember what they have eaten

    Am J Clin Nutr.

    (1997)
  • S.D. Baxter et al.

    “How do you remember you ate…?”a Delphi technique study to identify retrieval categories from fourth-grade children

    J Am Diet Assoc.

    (1997)
  • S.D. Baxter et al.

    Impact of gender, ethnicity, meal component, and time interval between eating and reporting on accuracy of fourth-graders’ self-reports of school lunch

    J Am Diet Assoc.

    (1997)
  • S.B. Domel et al.

    Accuracy of fourth- and fifth-grade students’ food records compared with school-lunch observations

    Am J Clin Nutr.

    (1994)
  • L.A. Lytle et al.

    Validating fourth-grade students’ self-report of dietary intakeresults from the 5 A Day Power Plus program

    J Am Diet Assoc.

    (1998)
  • The Surgeon General's Report on Nutrition and Health. Washington, DC: US Dept of Health and Human Services; 1988. DHHS...
  • Diet and HealthImplications for Reducing Chronic Disease Risk

    (1989)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text