Dissociable brain regions process object meaning and object structure during picture naming

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00083-5Get rights and content

Abstract

Although imaging studies have indicated that the fusiform gyrus is important in tasks of picture naming, whether this region encodes an object's structure or its meaning is not clear. We used positron emission tomography to examine cerebral blood flow (CBF) changes in response to a picture naming task that varied on two dimensions: familiarity (or difficulty: hard vs easy) and category (tools vs animals). Results show that although familiarity effects are present in the frontal and left lateral posterior temporal cortex, they are absent from the fusiform gyrus. This provides strong evidence that the processing carried out in the fusiform gyrus relates to an object's structure, not to its meaning, and that the left posterior middle temporal gyrus instantiates in part the semantic network that represents the object's meaning.

Introduction

The ability to identify and name visually presented objects involves multiple levels of progressively complex processing. Studies of individuals with focal brain lesions suggest that (in right-handers), focal left frontal lesions produce an impairment in object and picture naming via disruption in the ability to access the sound form of the words [7]. A similar naming deficit has also been induced by electrical stimulation of the basal temporal area in epileptic patients [6], [30], [31]. Brain-damaged individuals with lesions of the left occipito-temporal or posterior left temporal regions, however, develop a naming deficit for quite different reasons; they have either deficits in deriving the complete visual perceptual representation of the object's structure, or are impaired in their ability to access the stored semantic representation of the object's meaning [7], [14], [26].

Brain imaging studies have helped to localise in some measure the brain structures that are most involved in these identification processes. The posterior brain structure most consistently found to be activated in picture naming tasks is the fusiform gyrus of both hemispheres [5], [25], [27], [28], [35], [41], [49], located on the ventral surface of the occipital and temporal lobes. There is conflicting data, however, regarding the level of processing instantiated in this brain region. Since the fusiform is activated during both silent naming of real objects [35] as well as passive viewing of nonsense objects, it has been assigned a role in the perception of object form (similar in function to the occipital cortex) or as the storage site of perceptual information concerning known forms. Kanwisher, Halgren, Chao and others [8], [19], [24], however, have found that the specific areas of the ventral surface of the brain can be selectively activated by different categories of objects. For example, faces have been found to selectively activate the lateral posterior fusiform, whereas artifacts produce greater activation in the medial fusiform. That distinctions based on the semantic category of the stimuli are reflected in this region seems to suggest that these regions store aspects of the object's meaning. Indeed, other studies have produced results suggesting that semantic information as opposed to structural information is stored in the fusiform area. For example, when subjects are asked to read or make semantic (i.e. category membership) decisions on words referring to objects, there is also activation of the fusiform region [33], [37], [41], [45]. Furthermore this area has been found to be activated during tasks demanding conceptual [45], [52] object colour [33] and real world location knowledge [11]. These results could also be taken to indicate that semantic features are stored in the fusiform region bilaterally. We would argue that these conflicting results have not resolved the localisation question. An alternative interpretation would implicate a top-down activation of regions involved in perceptual and structural processing by higher order ‘semantic’ areas, an explanation invoked to explain category-specific activation of occipital cortex by animal pictures [35] or by visual imagery [29].

How might the neural basis of these different levels of representation be disentangled? In positron emission tomography (PET) studies of other cognitive domains, processing of more difficult or less familiar stimuli resulted in much greater focal changes in cerebral blood flow, reflecting the necessity of recruiting larger regions of relevant cortex for the task [23], [47], [55]. We hypothesised that the naming of visually presented items with weaker semantic representations (i.e. less familiar items) would impose a greater processing load on exactly those regions of cortex instantiating a semantic rather than a purely visual perceptual or structural level of representation. We therefore have used positron emission tomography to determine which areas of the brain were activated in picture naming while orthogonally controlling two factors: familiarity and category. The two categories contrasted were animals (which included birds and marine life) and manipulable tools. For both of these semantic categories the subjects were presented with pictures of highly familiar items (easy blocks) and of known but less familiar items (hard blocks). Visual complexity was similar for all four blocks. Subjects were asked to name the picture aloud, while undergoing PET scanning with the bolus H215O methodology. Standard subtraction images were subsequently created by comparing CBF during picture naming with two baseline conditions (presentation of plus signs and presentation of abstract designs). Subtraction images were also created by comparing the different picture naming scans with each other. In addition, regression maps were created in which each individual's behavioural performance (accuracy of picture naming) was regressed against cerebral blood flow for that particular block of pictures. This novel approach, pioneered by Paus et al. for sensory stimuli [44] supplied a powerful corollary approach to data analysis, since the regression map made no assumptions regarding the validity of any subtraction condition, and strictly evaluated which areas were more active during more effortful naming.

Section snippets

Subjects

Fifteen elderly subjects, seven men and eight women, without any history of neurological illness, participated in the experiment. Mean age ±S.D. of the participants was 73.5±7.4 years. Their mean years of education ±S.D. was 12.5±3.2. Mean accuracy (% correct ±S.D.) on the Boston Naming Test was 90.0±10.0. All subjects signed the appropriate informed consents, and the study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Montreal Neurological Institute.

Procedure

Subjects underwent six scans during which they

Behavioural data

The mean response naming accuracy scores (% correct ±S.D.) during scanning were: easy animals 93.5±9.9; easy tools 95.3±5.6; hard animals 57.3±23.1; hard tools 70.2±18.4. A two-way repeated-measures ANOVA carried out on the subjects’ picture naming accuracy revealed a main effect of object familiarity (F=77.3, P<0.001), confirming that subjects were more accurate on the easy blocks than on the hard blocks. There was also a main effect of picture category (F=5.0, P<0.05) and a significant

The effect of familiarity on the naming task

In this study we sought to determine the regions of the brain most involved in the semantic aspects of picture naming. In order to do this we looked for the locations where the effect of object familiarity was most evident. On both of the hard blocks, when the subjects were required to name less familiar objects, naming accuracy was significantly reduced as we had anticipated on the basis of the norming study. Due to the fixed presentation rate, however, the subjects in the PET study made many

Conclusion

We found that while category effects were present in the fusiform and occipital gyri, familiarity effects were manifested in the left middle temporal gyrus and prefrontal cortex. We propose that the lack of familiarity effects in the fusiform gyrus indicates that this region instantiates the visual ‘structural description’ of objects whereas the middle temporal region at least partially instantiates the semantic network that represents the object's meaning. Furthermore, we found that general

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by MRC grant no. MT-10450 to H. Chertkow, a chercheur-boursier clinicien senior award to H. Chertkow from the Fonds de la Recherche en Santé du Québec, and a post-doctoral award to C. Whatmough from the Canadian Institute for Health Research. We are grateful to the staff of the McConnel Brain Imaging Centre (BIC) of the Montreal Neurological Institute for technical support. This work would not be possible without the image analysis software of the BIC developed under

References (60)

  • T. Allison et al.

    Human extrastriate visual cortex and the perception of faces, words, numbers, and colors

    Cerebral Cortex

    (1994)
  • D.F. Benson

    Neurologic correlates of anomia

  • S.Y. Bookheimer et al.

    Regional cerebral blood flow during object naming and word reading

    Human Brain Mapping

    (1995)
  • T. Burnstine et al.

    Characterization of the basal temporal language area in patients with left temporal lobe epilepsy

    Neurology

    (1990)
  • D. Caplan

    Language: Structure, Processing, and Disorders

    (1992)
  • L.L. Chao et al.

    Attribute-based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects

    Nature Neuroscience

    (1999)
  • D.L. Collins et al.

    Automatic 3D intersubject registration of MR volumetric data in standardized Talairach space

    Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography

    (1994)
  • T. Crook et al.

    The misplaced-objects task: a brief test for memory dysfunction in the aged

    Journal of the American Geriatrics Society

    (1979)
  • J.B. Demb et al.

    Semantic encoding and retrieval in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: a functional MRI study of task difficulty and process specificity

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (1995)
  • Evans AC, Marrett S, Torrescorzo J, Ku S, Collins L. PET-MRI correlation using a volume-of-interest (VOI) atlas....
  • M.J. Farah

    Visual Agnosia: Disorders of Object Recognition and What They Tell Us about Normal Vision

    (1990)
  • J.A. Fiez et al.

    PET activation of posterior temporal regions during auditory word presentation and verb generation

    Cerebral Cortex

    (1996)
  • J.D.E. Gabrieli et al.

    Functional magnetic resonance imaging of semantic memory processes in the frontal lobes

    Psychological Science

    (1996)
  • E. Halgren et al.

    Location of human face-selective cortex with respect to retinotopic areas

    Human Brain Mapping

    (1999)
  • J. Hart et al.

    Delineation of single-word semantic comprehension deficits in aphasia, with anatomical correlation

    Annals of Neurology

    (1990)
  • J. Hart et al.

    Selective interference with the representation of size in the human by direct cortical electrical stimulation

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (1992)
  • G.W. Humphreys et al.

    An interactive activation approach to object processing: effects of structural similarity, name frequency, and task in normality and pathology

    Memory

    (1995)
  • N. Kanwisher et al.

    The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (1997)
  • N. Kanwisher et al.

    A locus in human extrastriate cortex for visual shape analysis

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (1997)
  • A. Kertesz

    The clinical spectrum and localisation of visual agnosia

  • Cited by (0)

    View full text