Elsevier

Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics

Volume 47, Issue 2, September–October 2008, Pages 277-289
Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics

Long-term effects of memory training in the elderly: A longitudinal study

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2007.08.010Get rights and content

Abstract

The effects of different types of memory training in young and older adults reported in a previous study [Cavallini, E., Pagnin, A., Vecchi, T., 2003. Aging and everyday memory: the beneficial effect of memory training. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 37, 241–257] were again investigated from a longitudinal perspective 2 years after the original memory training sessions. The authors retested the original participants to measure the long-term effectiveness of two mnemonic strategies: the loci technique and strategic training. Three groups of participants (13 adults, M = 24.1, 13 younger elderly, M = 64.2 and 13 older elderly, M = 74.4) were tested using a battery of seven tasks and four questionnaires, to evaluate memory performance and metamemory variables. The three age groups and the two trainings showed similar results on memory performance. Long-term effects were found only on two memory tasks, both were highly related to everyday life showing that, without additional practice, memory performance tended to go back to the original level. Moreover, the beneficial effects of the previous training sessions were particularly evident for older adults in metamemory knowledge and for strategic training in memory complaints. Results partially support the durability of memory training in improving memory performance.

Introduction

The benefits of strategy training for improving older adults’ memory performance have frequently been supported by experimental research (for a review, see Verhaeghen et al., 1992). However it is not clear yet whether the benefits of the training can be maintained throughout the years.

Usually researches on memory training (e.g., Yesavage et al., 1983, Scogin et al., 1985) are interested on short-term effects. On the contrary we know little about the long-term effects, and the literature on this topic is not homogenous. Scogin and Bienas (1988) evaluated long-term effects after 3 years from memory training sessions, both considering memory performance and complaints. After the follow-up, older adults’ performance returned to baseline levels and memory complaints did not change across this period of time. However, other researchers (Kliegl et al., 1990, Neely and Bäckman, 1993a, Neely and Bäckman, 1993b) reported long-term effects due to training even after some years. Oswald et al. (2002) demonstrated long-term beneficial effects up to 5 years after baseline examination. A similar trend was found by Ball et al. (2002) after a 2-year follow-up period.

These contradictory results on long-term effects may be explained on the basis of the difficulty for participants to re-use learned strategies in everyday life (Herrmann and Searleman, 1992). Elderly subjects often do not apply learned strategies after training because their employment in daily life requires great practice and exercise (McEvoy and Moon, 1988), or, alternatively, they tend to modify the learned strategies using them in a personal fashion (Anschutz et al., 1985, Anschutz et al., 1987). As a consequence, only subjects who incorporated trained skills into their repertoire of strategies and use them for the everyday learning were likely to exhibit long-term training effects (Anschutz et al., 1985). Nevertheless, a crucial aspect for analyzing long-term effects is related to spontaneous practice after the follow-up period. Meyer et al. (1989) introduced older adults to the use of an organizational strategy in order to learn a text. This strategy allows participants to read the text whilst at the same time discovering the structure of each passage, which proved to be useful in maintaining long-term gains. The beneficial effect of this type of training could also be associated with the fact that it could be easily employed in everyday memory practice such as reading a newspaper or a book (Dunlosky and Hertzog, 1998).

Regarding the effect of memory training on metamemory variables it seems that memory beliefs are resistant to modification (Scogin et al., 1985, McDonald-Miszczak et al., 1995, Pearman and Storandt, 2004) if training is focused on memory performance only (Verhaeghen et al., 1992). Different considerations concern metamemory knowledge since, compared to memory beliefs, the former can be gained through personal experience. Troyer (2001) demonstrated that memory knowledge, metamemory and objective memory performance can be effectively improved in old age through appropriate training.

In a previous study we investigated the effects of two different types of memory training in improving memory functions in old age by taking into account ecological and laboratory memory tasks and metamemory questionnaires (Cavallini et al., 2003). The data set of our previous experiment confirmed the validity of both memory trainings in improving memory performance and in reducing complaints about memory problems. Furthermore, our study showed that participants were able to re-use learned strategies in transfer tasks, thus improving their performance.

In the present paper, our aim is to evaluate the beneficial long-term effects of memory training in young and old people. Subjects who participated in a previous training regime (Cavallini et al., 2003) were re-tested 2 years after the training sessions. The study takes into account different training effects in both experimental and everyday memory tasks (Verhaeghen et al., 1992), whilst at the same time assesses metamemory knowledge and beliefs.

Section snippets

Method

We employed a battery of ecological and laboratory tasks together with four questionnaires associated with memory beliefs, metacognitive knowledge and memory complaints. Furthermore, the original study included two different types of memory training (loci and strategic training) and this aspect was also evaluated.

Results

Data were analyzed in a 3 × 3 × 2 repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA), with age (three groups: adult, younger elderly and older elderly) and training (two levels: loci and strategic) as between-subjects factors, and phase (three levels: test, retest-1 and retest-2) as within-subject factor. The comparisons of the factor main values were performed by means of a post hoc procedure. In particular, Tukey's test with 0.05 significance level was applied according to Keppel (1991).

Because the

Discussion

The present study examined the long-term effects of memory training after a follow-up period of 2 years. Our previous published study (Cavallini et al., 2003) showed an immediate improvement due to memory training in ecological tasks, suggesting the importance of teaching strategies or techniques. Unfortunately, this result was not found 2 years after training. In fact, the long-term effects observed during the follow-up period were not homogeneous across tasks.

Only in the face/name memory test

References (29)

  • E. Cavallini et al.

    Aging and everyday memory: the beneficial effect of memory training

    Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr.

    (2003)
  • A. Sunderland et al.

    Do laboratory tests predict everyday memory? A neuropsicological study

    J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav.

    (1983)
  • L. Anschutz et al.

    Maintenance and generalization of mnemonics for grocery shopping by older adults

    Exp. Aging Res.

    (1985)
  • L. Anschutz et al.

    Remembering mnemonics: a three-year follow-up on the effects of mnemonics training in elderly adults

    Exp. Aging Res.

    (1987)
  • K. Ball et al.

    Advanced cognitive training for independent and vital elderly study group, effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults. A randomized controlled trial

    J. Am. Med. Assoc.

    (2002)
  • P.B. Baltes et al.

    Psychological perspectives on successful aging: the model of selective optimization with compensation

  • G.H. Bower

    Analysis of a mnemonic device

    Am. Sci.

    (1970)
  • E. Cavallini et al.

    The rehabilitation of memory in old age: effects of mnemonics and metacognition in strategic training

    Clin. Gerontol.

    (2002)
  • C. Cornoldi et al.

    Cosa pensano gli anziani della loro memoria [What elderly people think about their memory]

  • R. De Beni et al.

    Fiducia nel proprio sistema di memoria e stili attributivi nell’anziano. Confronti tra differenti età e diversi contesti abitativi [Memory self-efficacy and attributional style in elderly people. Comparison among different ages and different living places]

    Ricerche di Psicologia

    (1996)
  • A. Derwinger et al.

    Design your own memory strategies! Self-generated strategy training versus mnemonic training in old age: an 8-month follow-up

    Neuropsychol. Rehabil.

    (2005)
  • J. Dunlosky et al.

    Training programs to improve learning in later adulthood: helping older adults educate themselves

  • M. Floyd et al.

    Effects of memory training on the subjective memory functioning and mental health of older adults: a meta-analysis

    Psychol. Aging

    (1997)
  • D. Herrmann et al.

    Memory improvement and memory theory in historical perspective

  • Cited by (35)

    • A controlled clinical efficacy trial of multimodal cognitive rehabilitation on episodic memory functioning in older adults with traumatic brain injury

      2021, Annals of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine
      Citation Excerpt :

      The effects of Face-name association were not as stable as hypothesized. Bottiroli et al. [27,34] suggested additional training sessions after the end of the intervention to facilitate long-term stability of the effects of memory training as well as specific instructions to individuals concerning the more suitable circumstances to apply the learned methods. The CEP intervention explicitly addressed the latter aspect, but additional or “booster” sessions and more diversified real-world practice circumstances could have been profitable.

    • Training older adults on Theory of Mind (ToM): Transfer on metamemory

      2015, Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics
      Citation Excerpt :

      We are, however, confident as existing research supports the existence of a certain degree of flexibility of metamemory. For example, Bottiroli and colleagues have shown that knowledge regarding memory can be modified through both metacognitive (Bottiroli et al., 2013) and strategic memory training (Bottiroli, Cavallini, & Vecchi, 2008). To summarize, the main aim of the present study was to test the effectiveness of a training on older adults’ ToM ability compared to a matched non-mentalistic contents training and a social-contact group.

    • Impact of metacognition and motivation on the efficacy of strategic memory training in older adults: Analysis of specific, transfer and maintenance effects

      2011, Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics
      Citation Excerpt :

      Further, the magnitude of the effects varies within studies. For example, Cavallini et al. (2003) found some improvement in everyday objective tasks associated with a reduction in number of complaints reported by the trained participants (young and older adults) in ecological context, maintained after two years (Bottiroli et al., 2008). However, no effects were found in level of memory self-efficacy.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text