Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 45, Issue 3, 15 April 2009, Pages 940-949
NeuroImage

Expectancy and treatment interactions: A dissociation between acupuncture analgesia and expectancy evoked placebo analgesia

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.025Get rights and content

Abstract

Recent advances in placebo research have demonstrated the mind's power to alter physiology. In this study, we combined an expectancy manipulation model with both verum and sham acupuncture treatments to address: 1) how and to what extent treatment and expectancy effects — including both subjective pain intensity levels (pain sensory ratings) and objective physiological activations (fMRI) — interact; and 2) if the underlying mechanism of expectancy remains the same whether placebo treatment is given alone or in conjunction with active treatment. The results indicate that although verum acupuncture + high expectation and sham acupuncture + high expectation induced subjective reports of analgesia of equal magnitude, fMRI analysis showed that verum acupuncture produced greater fMRI signal decrease in pain related brain regions during application of calibrated heat pain stimuli on the right arm. We believe our study provides brain imaging evidence for the existence of different mechanisms underlying acupuncture analgesia and expectancy evoked placebo analgesia. Our results also suggest that the brain network involved in expectancy may vary under different treatment situations (verum and sham acupuncture treatment).

Introduction

Embedded in biomedical science are many sets of related dichotomies: body or mind, objective or subjective, biology or belief, specific or non-specific, and pharmacology or placebo (Kirmayer, 1988). In treatment, these effects are generally considered additive (Kirsch, 2000, Kleijnen et al., 1994). In fact, the fundamental assumption of the randomized controlled trial is that a medication or procedure demonstrates efficacy by producing a statistically significant additive value above a placebo effect (Kaptchuk, 1998). Taking pain relief as an example, it is believed that analgesic treatments consist of two components: the treatment specific effect from the physical or pharmacological interventions, and the non-specific, or placebo, component. The latter is believed to be induced by the psychosocial context in which the treatment is given including expectations of therapeutic benefit (Colloca and Benedetti, 2005, Kleijnen et al., 1994).

Acupuncture has been studied in almost one thousand different randomized controlled trials (RCTs) (Ernst, 2006, Kaptchuk, 2000, Linde et al., 2001). The results of these trials have been contradictory or equivocal. In these trials, it is not uncommon for “placebo/sham acupuncture/minimal acupuncture” controls to induce positive therapeutic effects on the same order of magnitude as verum (genuine) acupuncture. In other studies, verum and sham groups often demonstrate superiority and clinical benefits over waiting list of conventional therapies controls (Brinkhaus et al., 2006, Haake et al., 2007, Kaptchuk, 2000, Kaptchuk, 2002a, Leibing et al., 2002, Linde et al., 2005, Melchart et al., 2005).

Acupuncture RCTs also suggest that expectancy, a crucial component of placebo (Benedetti, 2008, Kirsch, 1985, Montgomery and Kirsch, 1997), plays an important role in acupuncture treatment efficacy (Kaptchuk, 2002b, Kaptchuk et al., 2006, Linde et al., 2007). Importantly, basic science research investigating acupuncture analgesia on experimental pain, (which has produced more consistently positive results for acupuncture than clinical trials), has implicated overlapping mechanisms involving endogenous opioids and CCK for example, in both acupuncture (Han, 2003, Mayer et al., 1977) and placebo analgesia (Benedetti et al., 1999, Levine et al., 1978). This information suggests that the clinical practice of acupuncture may utilize some of the same endogenous analgesic mechanisms elicited by placebo treatments. Taken together, these findings beg the question — do the same mechanisms underlie acupuncture and placebo analgesia?

In parallel, advances in brain imaging provide scientists with new tools for investigating the neurobiological mechanisms underlying both placebo/nocebo and acupuncture treatment. Studies in placebo and nocebo research have shown the dramatic power of the mind to alter physiology and underlying brain circuitry (Bingel et al., 2006, Colloca et al., 2008b, Craggs et al., 2007, Kong et al., 2008, Kong et al., 2006a, Lieberman et al., 2004, Petrovic et al., 2002, Price et al., 2007, Scott et al., 2007, Scott et al., 2008, Wager et al., 2004, Wager et al., 2007, Zubieta et al., 2005). Investigations of acupuncture also demonstrate complicated brain networks involved in acupuncture stimulation (Dougherty et al., 2008, Fang et al., in press, Fang et al., 2004, Hui et al., 2000, Hui et al., 2005, Kong et al., 2007b, Kong et al., 2009, Kong et al., 2002, Li et al., 2004, Li et al., 2006, Napadow et al., 2007, Napadow et al., 2006, Napadow et al., 2004, Pariente et al., 2005, Wu et al., 1999, Wu et al., 2002, Yoo et al., 2004) and acupuncture analgesia (Zhang et al., 2003a,b). In contrast, little empirical work has directly examined the interactions between the physiological components of interventions and such non-specific factors as expectancy. For example, do acupuncture analgesia and placebo analgesia work through the same brain networks? Is the underlying mechanism of expectancy the same whether placebo treatment is given alone or in conjunction with active treatment?

One difficulty in studying placebo analgesia using healthy volunteers and experimentally-induced pain is that the placebo effects are fewer and smaller than those in genuine clinical pain studies (Beecher, 1959, Charron et al., 2006, Shapiro and Shapiro, 1997). In a previous study (Kong et al., 2006a), using a well-established expectancy/conditioning manipulation to increase expectation of analgesia, we found that placebo analgesia evoked by sham acupuncture, as indicated by subjective pain rating reductions, was significantly greater on placebo sites than on the control sites on the same right arm where subjects were not expecting an analgesic effect. The magnitude of this analgesia effect is comparable with the magnitude of the verum acupuncture analgesia effect observed in another study from our group (Kong et al., 2005).

In this current study, we combined the same expectancy/conditioning manipulation model to investigate whether the patterns of neural activation underlying the modulation of pain perception by verum acupuncture analgesia and expectancy evoked placebo analgesia can be distinguished using fMRI. In this experiment, subjects were randomized into four groups, receiving verum or sham acupuncture paired with either high or low expectancy/conditioning manipulation. Then analgesic response to experimental heat pain applied on the right forearm was tested. In each group, we also included a within-subject control. Subjects were told that if they responded to the acupuncture treatment they would experience analgesia only on the treated side of their arm, but not on the untreated side of their arm (Fig. 1). Forty-eight subjects (12 in each group) completed the experiment. In this manuscript, we present the data from two high expectancy groups (real and sham acupuncture) to investigate the underlying mechanisms of verum acupuncture and expectancy/conditioning induced placebo analgesia. Data from the low expectancy groups will be presented in a separate manuscript.

Section snippets

Subjects

Seventy-seven healthy, right-handed subjects enrolled in this experiment, which was said to be a study of acupuncture analgesia. All subjects were naïve to acupuncture. Experiments were conducted with the written consent of each subject and approval by the Massachusetts General Hospital's Institutional Review Board. All subjects were debriefed at the end of the experiment.

Procedures for the delivery and assessment of noxious thermal stimuli

Subjects were recruited to participate in two behavioral testing sessions and one fMRI scanning session. Each session was

Subjects

Forty-eight of seventy-seven consenting volunteers completed the study and were used for data analysis (average age 26.4 ± 4.9; 24 males). Twelve subjects did not fit the criteria for continued inclusion in the study (average ratings for HIGH pain were not greater than average ratings for LOW pain, or IP ratings on the radial and ulnar sides of their right arm were not approximately equivalent), eleven voluntarily withdrew, two could not tolerate the heat pain in Session 1, one could not tolerate

Discussion

In this study, we combined an expectancy manipulation procedure with both verum and sham acupuncture to investigate the brain networks involved in acupuncture analgesia, expectancy/conditioning induced placebo analgesia and their interrelationship. Our results showed that, although acupuncture analgesia and placebo analgesia show comparable magnitudes of behavioral efficacy, each has a different pattern of associated brain activation. Our results show that relatively different brain networks

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by PO1-AT002048 to Bruce Rosen from National Center for Complimentary and Alternative Medicine (NCCAM), R21AT00949 to Randy Gollub from NCCAM, KO1AT003883 to Jian Kong from NCCAM, K24AT004095 to Ted Kaptchuk from NCCAM, M01-RR-01066 and UL1 RR025758-01 for Clinical Research Center Biomedical Imaging Core from National Center for Research Resources (NCRR), P41RR14075 for Center for Functional Neuroimaging Technologies from NCRR and the MIND Institute.

References (104)

  • GracelyR.H. et al.

    Ratio scales of sensory and affective verbal pain descriptors

    Pain

    (1978)
  • GracelyR.H. et al.

    Validity and sensitivity of ratio scales of sensory and affective verbal pain descriptors: manipulation of affect by diazepam

    Pain

    (1978)
  • HanJ.S.

    Acupuncture: neuropeptide release produced by electrical stimulation of different frequencies

    Trends Neurosci.

    (2003)
  • HuiK.K. et al.

    The integrated response of the human cerebro-cerebellar and limbic systems to acupuncture stimulation at ST 36 as evidenced by fMRI

    NeuroImage

    (2005)
  • KaptchukT.J.

    Powerful placebo: the dark side of the randomised controlled trial

    Lancet

    (1998)
  • KirschI.

    Are drug and placebo effects in depression additive?

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2000)
  • KleijnenJ. et al.

    Placebo effect in double-blind clinical trials: a review of interactions with medications

    Lancet

    (1994)
  • KleinhenzJ. et al.

    Randomised clinical trial comparing the effects of acupuncture and a newly designed placebo needle in rotator cuff tendinitis

    Pain

    (1999)
  • KongJ. et al.

    Psychophysical outcomes from a randomized pilot study of manual, electro, and sham acupuncture treatment on experimentally induced thermal pain

    J. Pain

    (2005)
  • KongJ. et al.

    Test–retest study of fMRI signal change evoked by electro-acupuncture stimulation

    NeuroImage

    (2007)
  • LangevinH.M. et al.

    Pathophysiological model for chronic low back pain integrating connective tissue and nervous system mechanisms

    Med. Hypotheses

    (2007)
  • LangevinH.M. et al.

    Tissue displacements during acupuncture using ultrasound elastography techniques

    Ultrasound Med. Biol.

    (2004)
  • LeibingE. et al.

    Acupuncture treatment of chronic low-back pain — a randomized, blinded, placebo-controlled trial with 9-month follow-up

    Pain

    (2002)
  • LevineJ.D. et al.

    The mechanism of placebo analgesia

    Lancet

    (1978)
  • LiG. et al.

    Cortical activations upon stimulation of the sensorimotor-implicated acupoints

    Magn. Reson. Imaging

    (2004)
  • LiebermanM.D. et al.

    The neural correlates of placebo effects: a disruption account

    NeuroImage

    (2004)
  • LindeK. et al.

    The impact of patient expectations on outcomes in four randomized controlled trials of acupuncture in patients with chronic pain

    Pain

    (2007)
  • MayerD.J. et al.

    Antagonism of acupuncture analgesia in man by the narcotic antagonist naloxone

    Brain Res.

    (1977)
  • McKayD. et al.

    The effects of alcohol and alcohol expectancies on subjective reports and physiological reactivity: a meta-analysis

    Addict. Behav.

    (1999)
  • MontgomeryG.H. et al.

    Classical conditioning and the placebo effect

    Pain

    (1997)
  • NapadowV. et al.

    Somatosensory cortical plasticity in carpal tunnel syndrome—a cross-sectional fMRI evaluation

    NeuroImage

    (2006)
  • NapadowV. et al.

    Hypothalamus and amygdala response to acupuncture stimuli in carpal tunnel syndrome

    Pain

    (2007)
  • ParienteJ. et al.

    Expectancy and belief modulate the neuronal substrates of pain treated by acupuncture

    NeuroImage

    (2005)
  • PetrovicP. et al.

    Imaging cognitive modulation of pain processing

    Pain

    (2002)
  • PriceD.D. et al.

    An analysis of factors that contribute to the magnitude of placebo analgesia in an experimental paradigm

    Pain

    (1999)
  • PriceD.D. et al.

    Placebo analgesia is accompanied by large reductions in pain-related brain activity in irritable bowel syndrome patients

    Pain

    (2007)
  • ScottD.J. et al.

    Individual differences in reward responding explain placebo-induced expectations and effects

    Neuron

    (2007)
  • StreitbergerK. et al.

    Introducing a placebo needle into acupuncture research

    Lancet

    (1998)
  • VoudourisN.J. et al.

    The role of conditioning and verbal expectancy in the placebo response

    Pain

    (1990)
  • WhiteP. et al.

    The placebo needle, is it a valid and convincing placebo for use in acupuncture trials? A randomised, single-blind, cross-over pilot trial

    Pain

    (2003)
  • WuM.T. et al.

    Neuronal specificity of acupuncture response: a fMRI study with electroacupuncture

    NeuroImage

    (2002)
  • BeecherH.K.

    Measurement of Subjective Responses: Quantitative Effects of Drugs

    (1959)
  • BenedettiF.

    Mechanisms of placebo and placebo-related effects across diseases and treatments

    Annu. Rev. Pharmacol. Toxicol.

    (2008)
  • BenedettiF. et al.

    Somatotopic activation of opioid systems by target-directed expectations of analgesia

    J. Neurosci.

    (1999)
  • BenedettiF. et al.

    Neurobiological mechanisms of the placebo effect

    J. Neurosci.

    (2005)
  • BenedettiF. et al.

    The biochemical and neuroendocrine bases of the hyperalgesic nocebo effect

    J. Neurosci.

    (2006)
  • BrinkhausB. et al.

    Acupuncture in patients with chronic low back pain: a randomized controlled trial

    Arch. Intern. Med.

    (2006)
  • CharronJ. et al.

    Direct comparison of placebo effects on clinical and experimental pain

    Clin. J. Pain

    (2006)
  • CollocaL. et al.

    Placebos and painkillers: is mind as real as matter?

    Nat. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2005)
  • CurranH.V. et al.

    Cognitive and subjective dose–response effects of acute oral Delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) in infrequent cannabis users

    Psychopharmacology (Berl.)

    (2002)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text