Elsevier

Neuropsychologia

Volume 48, Issue 2, January 2010, Pages 353-365
Neuropsychologia

Reviews and perspectives
Split fovea theory and the role of the two cerebral hemispheres in reading: A review of the evidence

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.08.021Get rights and content

Abstract

Split fovea theory proposes that when the eyes are fixated within a written word, visual information about the letters falling to the left of fixation is projected initially to the right cerebral hemisphere while visual information about the letters falling to the right of fixation is projected to the left cerebral hemisphere. The two parts of the word must be re-united before the word can be recognised. Bilateral projection theory proposes instead that visual information is projected simultaneously to both hemispheres provided that it falls within the fovea (defined as the central 2–3°). On this more traditional account, no interhemispheric transfer would be required in order to read a word presented within the fovea. We review the evidence in support of split fovea theory and consider some of the objections that have been raised. We argue that a split fovea affects the reading of words at fixation, something that must be recognised and accounted for by cognitive, computational and neural models of reading.

Introduction

The essence of split fovea theory (SFT) is that the fovea is anatomically and functionally divided down the middle, with all visual information that originates to the left of fixation projecting initially to the right cerebral hemisphere while all visual information that originates to the right of fixation projects first to the left cerebral hemisphere. In contrast, the longer-established bilateral projection theory (BPT) proposes that while information presented in the left and right visual fields outside the fovea projects to the right and left hemispheres respectively, foveal information (usually taken to be the central 2–3°) is projected simultaneously to both hemispheres. That would include all the letters in a centrally fixated word that falls within the fovea.

When skilled readers are processing connected text, some words are skipped, but most are fixated. When a word is fixated, the fixation point typically falls about one-third of the way into the written word (O’Regan and Jacobs, 1992, Rayner, 1998). If the BPT is correct then, under normal reading conditions, the whole of a word that is contained within the fovea will be projected to visual areas in both hemispheres, including the language-dominant hemisphere from which word recognition can proceed. Word recognition will include access to the word's meaning (semantics) and its spoken form (phonology). If, however, the SFT is correct, then those letters in a centrally fixated word that fall to the right of fixation will be projected initially to visual areas in the left hemisphere while those letters that fall to the left of fixation will be projected first to visual areas in the right hemisphere. The two parts of the word will need to be brought together for identification, which would presumably involve the transfer of those letters projected to the non-dominant hemisphere across the corpus callosum to the language-dominant hemisphere. In readers with left hemisphere language dominance, which is the majority of readers, that would involve transfer of letters that fell to the left of fixation from the right hemisphere to the left hemisphere, where they could be re-united with the letters that fell to the right of the original fixation. If the SFT provides a better account of foveal processing of words and other visual stimuli, then researchers will need to explore the implications for cognitive, computational and brain-based models of reading, including how and where the left and right parts of a fixated word are re-united, and what problems might arise as a result of deficient callosal transfer (cf. Brysbaert, 1994, Brysbaert, 2004, Ellis, 2004, Ellis, 2009, Henderson et al., 2007, Lavidor and Walsh, 2004; Monaghan & Shillcock, 2008; Shillcock et al., 2000, Whitney, 2001, Whitney and Cornelissen, 2008).

There are three main lines of evidence relevant to the issue of whether the human fovea is or is not split, and whether any putative split affects visual word recognition. The first line of evidence comes from research involving patients with loss of vision in one or other visual hemifield (hemianopia). SFT predicts that when vision is lost in one visual hemifield, that loss will extend right up to the midline, even within the fovea. In contrast, because the BPT proposes that the fovea projects to both hemispheres, the BPT predicts that the fovea should be spared in hemianopic patients in whom only one visual field is damaged. The second line of evidence relevant to whether the human fovea is split or not concerns so-called ‘split-brain’ patients whose cerebral hemispheres have been surgically disconnected, either wholly or partially. SFT predicts that the problems such patients experience in transferring visual information between hemispheres as a result of the severing of callosal fibres should mean that one hemisphere will be unable to access visual information falling in the opposite visual field, even when that input falls within the fovea. The BPT theory makes the opposing prediction that because the fovea projects in its entirety to both hemispheres, each hemisphere will retain full access to foveal information even when extrafoveal information can no longer be transmitted between hemispheres. The third and final line of evidence comes from studies of word recognition in normal, healthy readers which explore parallels between the processing of whole words in the left or right visual fields (beyond the fovea) with the processing of the left and right halves of centrally fixated words. We will review each of these lines of evidence in turn, paying attention to the various points raised by Jordan and Paterson (2009) in their critical assessment of research in this area, an assessment which comes down in favour of BPT over SFT. Other reviews of the evidence, including more extensive coverage of older research, can be found in Brysbaert, 1994, Brysbaert, 2004, Ellis (2004), Leff (2004), and Lavidor and Walsh (2004). We should note that while our primary concern is with the implications of a split fovea for understanding reading processes, some of the crucial studies in the three different areas employed simple, nonverbal stimuli such as small dots or geometric figures. The importance of those studies for discriminating between BPT and SFT is such, however, that we will include them in this review.

Section snippets

Evidence from hemianopia

A basic source of evidence commonly cited in favour of BFT and against SFT was published by Huber (1962) who reasoned that by examining patients with hemianopia (i.e., loss of vision in one visual field as a consequence of unilateral ablation of primary visual cortex), he could determine the amount of central overlap (if any) between the left visual field (LVF) and the right visual field (RVF). Such overlap would be interpreted as indicating the region from which there is bilateral projection

Evidence from split-brain patients

This brings us to our second line of evidence for SFT which comes from the study of ‘split-brain’ patients. If the fovea is split, so that interhemispheric communication via the corpus callosum is required for foveal word recognition, a straightforward prediction is that central word recognition should no longer be possible in split-brain patients who have had their corpus callosum sectioned (provided that their eyes are not allowed to wander to one side of a word or the other, allowing whole

Evidence from normal readers

The evidence from hemianopia and split-brain patients strongly suggests the need for callosal transfer when processing foveal stimuli that cross the vertical meridian, including written words. The evidence is less than optimal, however, for drawing firm conclusions about the need for interhemispheric communication in normal word recognition. The hemianopia evidence is based on studies using simple, non-alphabetic stimuli, and only one of the split-brain studies used stimuli that were small

Re-evaluating the criticisms directed against SFT

For a long time, SFT has been criticized only by reference to the empirical evidence provided by Huber (1962), Stone et al. (1973), Bunt and Minckler (1977), Bunt et al. (1977), and Leventhal et al. (1988). Although the critics usually include more recent articles (e.g., Gazzaniga, 2000, Lindell and Nicholls, 2003) to support their claims, these articles are simply reviews based on the older material. In section 2 (above) we gave our reasons for believing that such evidence is not incompatible

Re-considering the usefulness of the Reicher–Wheeler task in hemispheric and split fovea research

Many different tasks have been used to investigate central word recognition (Grigorenko and Naples, 2008, Lupker, 2008). Most of those tasks have then been borrowed by researchers interested in the word recognition capabilities of the two cerebral hemispheres (see Banich, 2004, Ellis, 2004). Jordan and colleagues have championed the use of the Reicher–Wheeler task in hemispheric research, including split-fovea research (e.g., Jordan et al., 1998, Jordan et al., 2000, Jordan et al., 2003, Jordan

Closing remarks

SFT is a relatively new arrival on the theoretical and empirical stage. There is much to be done before we will be in a position to know whether SFT, BPT or some third alternative provides the best explanation of the mapping between the retinae and the brain, and of the processing of stimuli that fall entirely or partially to the left or right of fixation. SFT has generated a range of novel and counterintuitive findings in relation to word recognition – observations that would not have been

References (73)

  • R. Fendrich et al.

    Evidence of foveal splitting in a commissurotomy patient

    Neuropsychologia

    (1989)
  • R. Fendrich et al.

    Nasotemporal overlap at the retinal vertical meridian: Investigations with a callosotomy patient

    Neuropsychologia

    (1996)
  • L. Henderson et al.

    Interhemispheric cooperation and non-cooperation during word recognition: Evidence for callosal transfer dysfunction in dyslexic adults

    Brain and Language

    (2007)
  • J.H.W. Hsiao et al.

    Neural correlates of foveal splitting in reading: Evidence from an ERP study of Chinese character recognition

    Neuropsychologia

    (2007)
  • A. Huber

    Homonymous hemianopia after occipital lobectomy

    American Journal of Ophthalmology

    (1962)
  • Z.R. Hunter et al.

    Visual half field experiments are a good measure of cerebral language dominance if used properly

    Neuropsychologia

    (2008)
  • M. Iacoboni et al.

    Hemispheric independence in word recognition: Evidence from unilateral and bilateral presentations

    Brain and Language

    (1996)
  • T.R. Jordan et al.

    Re-evaluating split-fovea processing in visual word recognition: A critical assessment of recent research

    Neuropsychologia

    (2009)
  • T.R. Jordan et al.

    Re-evaluating split-fovea processing in word recognition: Effects of word length

    Cortex

    (2009)
  • M. Lavidor et al.

    Mixed-case effects in lateralized word recognition

    Brain and Cognition

    (2001)
  • M. Lavidor et al.

    Word length and orthographic neighborhood size effects in the left and right cerebral hemispheres

    Brain and Language

    (2002)
  • M. Lavidor et al.

    Orthographic neighborhood effects in the right but not in the left cerebral hemisphere

    Brain and Language

    (2002)
  • M. Lavidor et al.

    Case alternation and length effects in the two cerebral hemispheres: A study of English and Hebrew

    Brain and Cognition

    (2002)
  • M. Lavidor et al.

    Evaluating a split processing model of visual word recognition: Effects of word length

    Cognitive Brain Research

    (2001)
  • M. Lavidor et al.

    Evaluating a split processing model of visual word recognition: Effects of orthographic neighborhood size

    Brain and Language

    (2004)
  • A. Leff

    A historical review of the representation of the visual field in primary visual cortex with special reference to the neural mechanisms underlying macular sparing

    Brain and Language

    (2004)
  • A.K. Lindell et al.

    Cortical representation of the fovea: Implications for visual half-field research

    Cortex

    (2003)
  • A. Miki et al.

    Functional magnetic resonance imaging in homonymous hemianopsia

    American Journal of Ophthalmology

    (1996)
  • P. Monaghan et al.

    Hemispheric dissociation and dyslexia in a computational model of reading

    Brain and Language

    (2008)
  • M. Perea et al.

    Lexical competition is enhanced in the left hemisphere: Evidence from different types of orthographic neighbors

    Brain and Language

    (2008)
  • D.D. Wheeler

    Processes in word recognition

    Cognitive Psychology

    (1970)
  • A.W. Young et al.

    Different methods of lexical access for words presented in the left and right visual fields

    Brain and Language

    (1985)
  • E. Zaidel

    Language in the right hemisphere following callosal disconnection

  • M.T. Banich

    Interaction between the hemispheres and its implications for the processing capacity of the brain

  • D.B. Boles

    Dissociated imageability, concreteness, and familiarity in lateralized word recognition

    Memory & Cognition

    (1983)
  • M. Brysbaert et al.

    Visual constraints on written word recognition: Evidence from the optimal viewing position effect

    Journal of Research in Reading

    (2005)
  • Cited by (51)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Andrew Ellis and Marc Brysbaert were members of the EU Marie Curie Research Training Network on Language and Brain.

    View full text