Anxiety, working memory, gender, and math performance

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2003.09.029Get rights and content

Abstract

This investigation evaluated the relations between visual and verbal working memory; state, trait, and math anxiety; gender; and applied and basic math performance in 100 adults. The design tested predictions regarding which subsystem of working memory is associated with both math anxiety and math performance. The study also tested whether the Processing Efficiency Theory and the Arousal-Performance Function apply to anxiety and performance in the math domain. In addition, gender differences in the relations between math anxiety and math performance were explored. Results indicated that math anxiety was the strongest predictor of both applied and basic math performance. However, this finding was moderated by the gender of the participants. Both visual and verbal working memory were found to be significant factors in accounting for the variance in math performance measured broadly, differing from findings in previous studies. Math anxiety appears to primarily impact visual working memory, contradicting previous findings that anxiety is primarily processed in verbal working memory and supporting the hypothesis that math anxiety does not function similarly to other types of anxiety. In addition, results supported the Processing Efficiency Theory but did not support the Arousal-Performance Function relating math anxiety and math performance.

Introduction

Math performance is considered to be an important aspect of academic achievement tests (e.g., the Woodcock–Johnson (Woodcock & Johnson, 1990); the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (Psychological Corporation, 1992); the Iowa Tests of Basic Skills (Hoover, Dunbar, & Frisbie, 2001); the SAT (College Board, 1995); the ACT (American College Testing Program, 1984); and the GRE (Education Testing Service, 1994)). In addition, math ability is a component of many intelligence theories (e.g., Gustafsson, 1984; Horn & Cattell, 1966; Thurstone, 1938), and intelligence measures (e.g., the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (Wechsler, 1981), the Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scale (Thorndike, Hagen, & Sattler, 1986); and the Woodcock–Johnson Test of Cognitive Abilities (Woodcock & Johnson, 1990)). Understanding which factors account for differences in math performance, then, has important implications for predicting performance on these measures.

Past research has investigated the underlying cognitive processes that contribute to individual differences in math ability, the most investigated of which appears to be working memory. Several studies have shown that the processes involved in working memory, namely, temporary retrieval, processing, and storage, explain much of the variance in math ability (e.g., Adams & Hitch, 1998; Ashcraft, 1995; Brainerd, 1983; Geary & Widaman, 1987; Hitch, 1978). Individuals who are more efficient and adept in carrying out these processes are likely to perform better on tests of math ability.

Another often-researched factor proposed to account for differences in math ability is gender. Theorists have argued about whether findings of an advantage for males over females are the result of an inherent difference in math ability or the influence of sex-based stereotypes (see Hyde, Fennema, Ryan, Frost, & Hopp, 1990, for a review). It is important to note that the effect sizes reported for the relation between gender and math performance have been small (see Hyde et al., 1990, for a meta-analysis, and Leahey & Guo, 2001, for examination of a large data set).

A third factor that appears to account for differences in math performance is anxiety. Hembree’s (1990) meta-analysis examining math anxiety found that female students consistently reported higher levels of math anxiety than their male counterparts. However, this higher level of anxiety was not found to hinder math performance in females. Rather, math performance in pre-college males was affected more by their levels of math anxiety than math performance in pre-college females. The present study addresses whether gender moderates anxiety’s relation to different types of math performance.

There are two general types of anxiety, trait and state. Individuals experiencing trait anxiety have a characteristic tendency to feel anxious across all types of situations (Sorg & Whitney, 1992). In contrast, individuals possessing state anxiety tend to experience it only in specific personally stressful or fearful situations.

The results of several studies demonstrate that both state and trait anxiety affect task performance (e.g., Leon & Revelle, 1985; MacLeod & Donnellan, 1993; Sorg & Whitney, 1992). In general, these studies have found that individuals with high trait anxiety show poorer performance on various tasks than low trait anxiety individuals. This difference tends to be exacerbated in a high state anxiety condition.

In addition to the two general types of anxiety, there exist more specific types of anxiety, including math and test anxiety. Hembree (1990) found that math anxiety is related to both trait and state anxiety, as well as to test anxiety. Although math anxiety and test anxiety were related, however, the amount of shared variance was moderate (r2=0.27), suggesting that math anxiety is not purely a consequence of anxiety over being tested.

Section snippets

The Arousal-Performance Function

Sorg and Whitney (1992) examined the effects of both trait and state anxiety on working memory. Results showed that those individuals with high trait anxiety suffered from working memory deficits and poorer performance when placed under the additional stress of a high state anxiety condition. Of additional interest was that a low level of performance was also observed in low trait anxiety individuals in the low state anxiety condition. Individuals with intermediate levels of arousal (low trait

Subjects

One hundred adults were recruited for this study. A total of 62 females and 38 males comprised the subject sample, ranging in age from 18 to 66. The sample consisted of 42 college students, 30 college graduates, 2 individuals with less than 2 years of college and no degree, and 26 individuals with no college education. This sample appears to be more representative than those used in previous studies of adult math performance, most of which have tested college students.

Measures

Math Performance Assessment

Results

Descriptive statistics for each of the measures are displayed in Table 1. Both of the math performance measures were designed to test age ranges from 4 to adult. Therefore, scores on each of the subtests were expected to fall in the high end of the range given that the sample consisted entirely of adults.

Intercorrelations between each of the measures are displayed in Table 2. Applied math performance was correlated with basic math performance, suggesting that the two types of math performance

Discussion

The results of this study indicate that math anxiety was the most significant factor in predicting variance in both applied and basic math performance. Individuals with high levels of math anxiety had lower scores on both the basic and applied math subtests. Additionally, after math anxiety, both verbal working memory (as measured by the reading span task) and visual working memory (as measured by the paper-folding task) appeared to be approximately equally important in predicting both types of

Conclusions

Math anxiety was found to be the most important predictor of math performance, followed by both verbal and visual working memory. Math anxiety does not function entirely like other types of anxiety, at least as related to math performance. The finding that math anxiety is primarily associated with visual working memory, rather than verbal working memory, contradicts previous results in which more limited math abilities were tested. In addition, the results of this study supported the Processing

Acknowledgements

We thank Barbara Maffett, who tested many of the subjects involved in the study and assisted in data entry. We also thank William Chaplin for his comments and suggested revisions to the manuscript.

References (37)

  • American College Testing Program (1984). American College Testing (ACT) Assessment Program....
  • M.H. Ashcraft

    Cognitive psychology and simple arithmetic: A review and summary of new directions

    Mathematical Cognition

    (1995)
  • M.H. Ashcraft et al.

    The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

    (2001)
  • J. Bichsel et al.

    Imaginal discovery, working memory, and intelligence

    Journal of Mental Imagery

    (1999)
  • C.J. Brainerd

    Young children’s mental arithmetic errors: A working memory analysis

    Child Development

    (1983)
  • M.G. Calvo et al.

    Compensatory reading strategies in test anxiety

    Anxiety, Stress, and Coping

    (1994)
  • College Board (1995). Admission officer’s handbook for the SAT Program. New York: College Entrance Examination...
  • K.H. Dew et al.

    Mathematics anxiety: Some basic issues

    Journal of Counseling Psychology

    (1983)
  • Cited by (152)

    • The link between math anxiety and performance does not depend on working memory: A network analysis study

      2022, Consciousness and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, there are inconsistencies in findings for the subsystems. While some studies have indicated a more pronounced effect of VSWM on math performance (Allen et al., 2019; Giofrè et al., 2018; McKenzie et al., 2003; Miller & Bichsel, 2004; St Clair-Thompson & Gathercole, 2006; Toll et al., 2016), others have demonstrated the influence of VWM (Ashkenazi & Danan, 2017; Hitch & McAuley, 1991; Wilson & Swanson, 2001), particularly with a high math-load (for a review, see Raghubar et al., 2010). Consistent with the latter line of research, we found WM was positively related to math performance (Raghubar et al., 2010), with math performance more strongly correlated with VWM than VSWM (Ashkenazi & Danan, 2017; Hitch & McAuley, 1991; Wilson & Swanson, 2001).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text