Elsevier

Physiology & Behavior

Volume 80, Issues 2–3, November 2003, Pages 317-325
Physiology & Behavior

Novel-object place conditioning in adolescent and adult male and female rats: effects of social isolation

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2003.08.003Get rights and content

Abstract

Elevated levels of novelty seeking are often seen during adolescence. Recent studies using a conditioned place preference (CPP) paradigm have shown that novelty may be rewarding for adult rats. The present study explored the impact of age, sex, and isolation stress on novelty seeking and novelty reward by assessing novel object-induced CPP in adolescent and adult male and female Sprague–Dawley rats housed either socially or in isolation. Responding to the novel objects during conditioning was higher in adolescent animals than adults, and was suppressed by social isolation only in adulthood, particularly among males. Novel object CPP was strong among adolescent males, whereas only socially isolated adult males demonstrated preference for the compartment paired with the novel objects. This age difference was not evident in females, with both adolescent and adult group-housed females, but not their isolated counterparts, showing novel-object place conditioning. These dissociations between novelty-directed behaviors during conditioning and novelty reward in the CPP paradigm support the suggestion that mechanisms underlying novelty seeking are separable from those involved in the rewarding effects of novelty. High levels of novelty seeking demonstrated by adolescents do not necessarily predict high rewarding properties of novelty, with the latter also being influenced by environmental and gender-related factors.

Introduction

Relative to adults and children, human adolescents exhibit an increased level of risk-taking behavior, associated with the motivation to experience multiple new and intense stimuli to attain potential rewards [1], [2]. The motivation to seek out new experiences, i.e., novelty seeking behavior, has been identified as a significant contributor to current and future drug use, multiple drug use, and later abuse [3], [4], [5], [6].

Given that adolescence is a time of acquiring new skills for survival away from parents, enhanced novelty seeking may have been evolutionally conserved for its adaptive value during this developmental period, contributing to exploration of novel areas and providing the opportunity to find new sources of food, water, and mates (see Ref. [7]). Although little explored to date, there is some experimental evidence that age may play a role in responding to novelty, with adolescent rodents being more responsive to novel stimuli than their adult counterparts. For instance, adolescent mice spent more time in a novel environment and had higher levels of novelty-induced activity than adults [8]. In a similar way, adolescent rats have been reported to be more active and to engage in more exploratory behaviors in novel situations than adults [9], [10].

Responding to novelty may also be influenced by social factors. In humans, a number of social variables, including social conformity, peer deviance, and social support influence novelty seeking behavior in adolescence [11]. Likewise, the presence of peers before testing enhanced responding to novelty in nonhuman primates, increasing exploration of novel objects relative to their socially deprived counterparts [12]. In contrast, rats reared in social isolation were more responsive to novelty relative to rats housed socially (see Ref. [13] for references and review), as indexed by increased locomotor activity [14], [15], enhanced preference for novel environments [16], and elevated levels of object exploration [17]. Such isolation-induced increases in object exploration were found to be age-specific, emerging only in animals socially deprived between 25 and 45 days of age and not by isolation initiated after P45 [18]. These findings suggest that responding to novelty of adolescent animals may be influenced by social factors in a way different from that of their adult counterparts.

Conditioned place preference (CPP), a widely used behavioral paradigm to measure drug reward in laboratory animals, has been employed recently for assessment of the rewarding properties of novel environmental stimuli. In these studies, socially isolated adult male rats received access to novel objects in one distinct environment (e.g., one compartment of a CPP apparatus containing distinct tactile and visual cues), while receiving exposure to a second distinct environment (the other compartment of the apparatus) in the absence of any novel objects. In a choice test, when exposed to the entire apparatus without novel objects, rats showed preference for the environment that had been previously paired with novelty [19], [20], [21]. Novelty of the objects is an important issue in this paradigm, since animals that received access to familiar objects in one of the compartments of the CPP apparatus did not show preference for that environment later on, and during conditioning they spent significantly less time interacting with the familiar object than their counterparts exposed to novel objects [21]. These results suggest that access to novel but not familiar objects is rewarding, findings that validate the use of this procedure for the assessment of novelty reward.

There are no reports as to whether novel stimuli would support place preference conditioning in adolescent animals, nor whether such conditioning would be influenced by sex and housing conditions in an age-specific manner. Therefore, the main objective of the present study was to evaluate how age, sex, and isolate housing influence the rewarding properties of novelty using the novel-object place conditioning paradigm developed by Bevins and Bardo [20].

Section snippets

Animals

Animals were derived from Sprague–Dawley VAF rats bred and reared in our colony at Binghamton University. Pups were housed until weaning with their parents in standard maternity cages with pine shavings as bedding material. All animals were housed in a temperature-controlled (22 °C) vivarium maintained on a 12-h/12-h illumination cycle (lights on at 0700 h) with ad libitum access to food (Purina Rat Chow, Lowell, MA) and water. Litters were culled to 10 pups (with a sex ratio of four pups of

Behavior during conditioning

In general, responding to the novel objects was higher in adolescent animals than in their adult counterparts. Adolescents started to explore the novel objects more quickly and spent more time interacting with them than adults. Levels of locomotor activity were also elevated in adolescent rats. Social deprivation decreased responding to novelty in adult but not adolescent animals, and males were more affected than females.

Latency to contact the novel objects did not differ as a function of day

Discussion

The results of the present study demonstrated that in general responding to novelty was higher in adolescent animals than in their adult counterparts. During conditioning, adolescents showed shorter latencies to contact the novel objects and spent more time interacting with them than adults. Age-related differences in responding to novelty were observed not only in males but also in female rats. High responding to the novel objects demonstrated by adolescents in the present study is in

References (53)

  • I.K. Wright et al.

    Resocialization of isolation-reared rats does not alter their anxiogenic profile on the elevated x-maze model of anxiety

    Physiol. Behav.

    (1991)
  • F.S. Hall et al.

    The effects of isolation-rearing on preference by rats for a novel environment

    Physiol. Behav.

    (1997)
  • B.J. Vastola et al.

    Nicotine-induced place preference in adolescent and adult rats

    Physiol. Behav.

    (2002)
  • J.O. Campbell et al.

    Cocaine and morphine-induced place conditioning in adolescent and adult rats

    Physiol. Behav.

    (2000)
  • G.H. Jones et al.

    Dopaminergic and serotonergic function following isolation rearing in rats: study of behavioural responses and postmortem and in vivo neurochemistry

    Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav.

    (1992)
  • C.A. Heidbreder et al.

    Behavioral, neurochemical and endocrinological characterization of the early social isolation syndrome

    Neuroscience

    (2000)
  • S. Cabib et al.

    Opposite imbalances between mesocortical and mesoaccumbens dopamine responses to stress by the same genotype depending on living conditions

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2002)
  • L.A. Galea et al.

    Sex differences in dendritic atrophy of CA3 pyramidal neurons in response to chronic restraint stress

    Neuroscience

    (1997)
  • D. Haleem et al.

    Adaptation of female rats to stress: shift to male pattern by inhibition of corticosterone synthesis

    Brain Res.

    (1988)
  • G. Kennett et al.

    Female rats are more vulnerable than males in an animal model of depression: the possible role of serotonin

    Brain Res.

    (1986)
  • C.J. Harmer et al.

    Isolation rearing enhances acquisition in a conditioned inhibition paradigm

    Physiol. Behav.

    (1998)
  • N. Wongwitdecha et al.

    Effects of social isolation rearing on learning in the Morris water maze

    Brain Res.

    (1996)
  • J. Besheer et al.

    Dopaminergic and cholinergic antagonism in a novel-object detection task with rats

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2001)
  • S.L. Andersen et al.

    Sex differences in dopamine receptors and their relevance to ADHD

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2000)
  • R.M. Trimpop et al.

    Comparing personality constructs of risk-taking behavior

    Pers. Individ. Differ.

    (1999)
  • M.E. Bates et al.

    The effects of sensation seeking needs on alcohol and marijuana use in adolescence

    Bull. Soc. Psychol. Addict. Behav.

    (1986)
  • Cited by (126)

    • Primary role for melatonin MT<inf>2</inf> receptors in the regulation of anhedonia and circadian temperature rhythm

      2021, European Neuropsychopharmacology
      Citation Excerpt :

      First the Area Under the Curve (AUC) for the 24 h CBT was obtained for the quiets’ weekday of the animal facility, Sundays 8 a.m. to Mondays 8 a.m. Second, mean temperatures during the night period (12 h, 8 pm- 8 am) were obtained for both standard conditions (food and water available) or during the sucrose intake test (sucrose solution and water available) in order to study mice sensitivity to reward exposure. Novel object-induced CPP test was applied with minor modifications (Douglas et al., 2003). A conventional conditioned place preference apparatus was used.

    • Rigor and reproducibility in rodent behavioral research

      2019, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text