Review
The social motivation theory of autism

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.02.007Get rights and content

The idea that social motivation deficits play a central role in Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) has recently gained increased interest. This constitutes a shift in autism research, which has traditionally focused more intensely on cognitive impairments, such as theory-of-mind deficits or executive dysfunction, and has granted comparatively less attention to motivational factors. This review delineates the concept of social motivation and capitalizes on recent findings in several research areas to provide an integrated account of social motivation at the behavioral, biological and evolutionary levels. We conclude that ASD can be construed as an extreme case of diminished social motivation and, as such, provides a powerful model to understand humans’ intrinsic drive to seek acceptance and avoid rejection.

Section snippets

Social motivation and social cognition: two competing accounts of autism

Over the past three decades, a number of theories have been put forward to account for the pervasive social impairments found in Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD). Among the various attempts, the idea of a core deficit in social cognition (theory of mind, or ToM, in particular; see Glossary) has become one of the most prominent accounts of ASD. Concomitantly, the impact of motivational factors on the development of social skills and social cognition has received little attention. Recently,

Behavioral level

Behavioral manifestations of humans’ social interest are of at least three kinds: (i) objects with social importance are prioritized by attention; (ii) social interactions are rewarding; (iii) interpersonal behaviors are influenced by the desire to maintain and enhance relationships. We now review interdisciplinary evidence supportive of this three-tiered disposition.

Social orienting. In very much the same way that negative signals (e.g. threats) capture attention, potentially beneficial or

Behavioral level

Social motivation models of ASD posit that early-onset impairments in social attention set in motion developmental processes that ultimately deprive the child of adequate social learning experiences, and that the resulting imbalance in attending to social and non-social stimuli further disrupts social skill and social cognitive development 46, 47, 48. As discussed in detail below, recent evidence demonstrates that social orienting, social seeking and liking, and social maintaining are all

What is the scope of the social motivation theory?

Although many questions remain (Box 3), the research reviewed here suggests that the social motivation theory provides a credible framework to account for social impairments in ASD. However, by concentrating on social deficits, the social motivation account faces similar shortcomings as the ToM account. Unlike non-social accounts (e.g. executive dysfunction or weak central coherence), both of these social theories indeed fall short of explaining non-social deficits in ASD, such as repetitive

Concluding remarks

The social world summons our attention like no other domain: social signals are prioritized by attention, interactions are intrinsically rewarding, and social maintaining permeates interpersonal behaviors. Social motivation is subserved by dedicated biological mechanisms and can be seen as an evolutionary adaptation to humans’ highly collaborative environment: by enhancing attention to social information, by rewarding social interactions, and by promoting the desire to effectively maintain

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the National Institute of Health NIMH R01MH073084 (R.T.S.) and R01MH080718 (E.S.B.), by the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation #66727 (R.T.S., G.K.), by the Pennsylvania Department of Health SAP # 4100042728 (R.T.S., C.C.) and 4100047863 (R.T.S., E.S.B.), and by a National Science Foundation Graduate Fellowship (V.T.). The authors wish to thank Nicolas Baumard, Francesca Happé and Hugo Mercier for stimulating discussion and debate on the ideas presented here.

Glossary

Audience effect
refers to the influence of the presence of a spectator on a subject's performance or decisions. This classic effect in social psychology has received robust experimental support. Behavioral economists have demonstrated that the presence of others enhances participants’ generosity in a range of games, such as the dictator game, the ultimatum game, and the public good game.
Overjustification effect
refers to the fact that extrinsic incentives, such as money, can undermine

References (107)

  • J.A. Bartz

    Social effects of oxytocin in humans: context and person matter

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2011)
  • V. Trezza

    The pleasures of play: pharmacological insights into social reward mechanisms

    Trends Pharmacol. Sci.

    (2010)
  • R. Schultz

    Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: the role of the amygdala and fusiform face area

    Int. J. Dev. Neurosci.

    (2005)
  • D.M. Riby et al.

    Viewing it differently: social scene perception in Williams syndrome and autism

    Neuropsychologia

    (2008)
  • M. Elsabbagh

    Infant neural sensitivity to dynamic eye gaze is associated with later emerging autism

    Curr. Biol.

    (2012)
  • J. Bachevalier et al.

    The orbitofrontal-amygdala circuit and self-regulation of social-emotional behavior in autism

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2006)
  • C.L. Masten

    An fMRI Investigation of responses to peer rejection in adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders

    Dev. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (2011)
  • S. Jacob

    Association of the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) in Caucasian children and adolescents with autism

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (2007)
  • P. Mehzabin et al.

    Self-assessed sexuality in young adults with High-Functioning Autism

    Res. Autism Spectr. Dis.

    (2011)
  • R. Baillargeon

    False-belief understanding in infants

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2010)
  • J.T. Cacioppo et al.

    Perceived social isolation and cognition

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2009)
  • S. Fletcher-Watson

    Rapid detection of person information in a naturalistic scene

    Perception

    (2008)
  • Y. Kikuchi

    Faces do not capture special attention in children with autism spectrum disorder: a change blindness study

    Child Dev.

    (2009)
  • T. Ro

    Changing faces: a detection advantage in the flicker paradigm

    Psychol. Sci.

    (2001)
  • D.G. Purcell et al.

    The face-detection effect: configuration enhances detection

    Atten. Percept. Psychophys.

    (1988)
  • T. Gliga

    Faces attract infants’ attention in complex displays

    Infancy

    (2009)
  • O. Rosa Salva

    The evolution of social orienting: evidence from chicks (gallus gallus) and human newborns

    PLoS ONE

    (2011)
  • Y. Jiang

    Processing of invisible stimuli: advantage of upright faces and recognizable words in overcoming interocular suppression

    Psychol. Sci.

    (2007)
  • B.Y. Hayden

    Economic principles motivating social attention in humans

    Proc. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci.

    (2007)
  • S. Bowles

    Policies designed for self-interested citizens may undermine ‘the moral sentiments’: evidence from economic experiments

    Science

    (2008)
  • F. Warneken et al.

    Extrinsic rewards undermine altruistic tendencies in 20-month-olds

    Dev. Psychol.

    (2008)
  • M.R. Leary et al.

    Belonging motivation: establishing, maintaining, and repairing relational value

  • C. Higgins

    Influence tactics and work outcomes: a meta analysis

    J. Organ. Behav.

    (2003)
  • V. Talwar

    White lie-telling in children for politeness purposes

    Int. J. Behav. Dev.

    (2007)
  • G. Fu et al.

    Social grooming in the kindergarten: the emergence of flattery behavior

    Dev. Sci.

    (2007)
  • H. Ross

    Shading the truth: self-serving biases in children's reports of sibling conflicts

    Merrill-Palmer Q.

    (2004)
  • J.L. Lakin

    The chameleon effect as social glue: evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry

    J. Nonverbal Behav.

    (2003)
  • T.L. Chartrand et al.

    The chameleon effect: the perception–behavior link and social interaction

    J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.

    (1999)
  • J.L. Lakin et al.

    Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport

    Psychol. Sci.

    (2003)
  • A. Lin

    Social and monetary reward learning engage overlapping neural substrates

    Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci.

    (2011)
  • G. Tabibnia et al.

    Fairness and cooperation are rewarding

    Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.

    (2007)
  • K. Izuma

    Processing of the incentive for social approval in the ventral striatum during charitable donation

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (2010)
  • J. Rilling

    Opposing BOLD responses to reciprocated and unreciprocated altruism in putative reward pathways

    Neuroreport

    (2004)
  • A.E. Guyer

    Amygdala and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex function during anticipated peer evaluation in pediatric social anxiety

    Arch. Gen. Psychiatry

    (2008)
  • D.P. Kennedy

    Personal space regulation by the human amygdala

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2009)
  • M.L. Spezio

    Amygdala damage impairs eye contact during conversations with real people

    J. Neurosci.

    (2007)
  • J. Hornak

    Changes in emotion after circumscribed surgical lesions of the orbitofrontal and cingulate cortices

    Brain

    (2003)
  • Y. Takayanagi

    Pervasive social deficits, but normal parturition, in oxytocin receptor-deficient mice

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.

    (2005)
  • M.E. Avale

    Prefrontal nicotinic receptors control novel social interaction between mice

    FASEB J.

    (2011)
  • M. Tomasello

    Human culture in evolutionary perspective

    Adv. Cult. Psychol.

    (2010)
  • Cited by (1324)

    • The basal forebrain serves social information processing

      2024, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text