Elsevier

Hormones and Behavior

Volume 50, Issue 3, September 2006, Pages 489-495
Hormones and Behavior

Increased anxiety-like behavior during the post-stress period in mice exposed to repeated restraint stress

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.06.007Get rights and content

Abstract

Mice exposed to repeated restraint (RR: 2 h of restraint on each of 3 consecutive days) lose weight and do not return to the weight of non-stressed controls after restraint ends. These mice also exhibit an exaggerated endocrine response to mild stressors in the post-stress period. To determine if other aspects of the stress response are altered, NIH Swiss mice were repeatedly restrained then evaluated for anxiety-like behavior in various behavioral tests. Twelve days after the end of RR half of the control and RR mice were subjected to the mild stress of an intraperitoneal injection of saline before placement in an elevated plus maze. RR mice not subjected to mild stress showed the same level of anxiety as the control and RR mice exposed to mild stress. Placement in a light–dark box 20 days after restraint also indicated an increase in anxiety-like behavior in RR mice that had not been exposed to mild stress. In contrast, RR mice displayed no increase in anxiety-like behavior in the defensive withdrawal apparatus and the marble burying test 6 and 17 days, respectively, after restraint. RR mice released more corticosterone than non-restrained controls exposed to defensive withdrawal or EPM apparatus although baseline corticosterone remained at control levels. These results suggest that RR induces an exaggeration of both endocrine and behavioral responses to subsequent mild stressors. This post-stress hypersensitivity to mild stress may contribute to the sustained reduction in the body weight of RR animals.

Introduction

Mice and rats exposed to 2 h of restraint each morning for 3 consecutive days (repeated restraint, RR) lose weight during the period of restraint (Harris et al., 2001). It has been shown that this initial weight loss is due to an increase in energy expenditure and a decrease in food intake on the days of restraint (Harris et al., 2006). Following RR, food intake and energy expenditure return to pre-stress levels and the animals gain weight at the same rate as controls, however, they do not return to the weight of the controls (Harris et al., 2001, Harris et al., 2006). Although baseline serum corticosterone levels and the daily nadir of corticosterone release remain normal, RR animals exhibit an exaggerated corticosterone response to novel mild stressors presented 12 days after the end of repeated restraint (Harris et al., 2004). Although much of the work with this model has been carried out with rats, mice exposed to RR also show the same sustained down-regulation of body weight and hyperresponsiveness to subsequent mild stressors (Harris et al., 2001, Harris et al., 2004). Others have reported similar increased sensitivity to subsequent stressors in animals that have been acutely stressed. For example, rats exposed to one or three daily sessions of footshock show an exaggerated corticosterone response to a stress applied 10 days later (Caggiula et al., 1989, Gomez et al., 2002, Servatius et al., 1994). The increased sensitivity of the endocrine response to stressors in the post-stress period has been associated with the impaired ability of the glucocorticoids to suppress hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis activity and a lowering of the threshold for the initiation of endocrine responses to stress (Buwalda et al., 1997, Harris et al., 2004).

Exposure to stress results in a myriad of responses, of which activation of the HPA axis (Cook, 2004), an increase in energy expenditure (Harris et al., 2006), and a decrease in feeding and appetite (Harris et al., 2002b) are only a few. Exposure to stress also results in behavioral changes. For example, a period of acute restraint stress has been shown to increase anxiety-like behavior in three well-established behavior tests, the elevated plus maze (EPM), the defensive withdrawal paradigm, and the light–dark box measured immediately after the end of the restraint stress (Harris et al., 2001, Harris et al., 2002a, MacNeil et al., 1997, Morilak et al., 2003, Smagin et al., 1996). These behavioral tests rely on the conflict between the rodent's natural aversion to illuminated, open spaces and a tendency to explore novel areas. In addition, there is evidence that exposure to an acute stressor can result in sustained increases in anxiety-like behavior. In 1992, van Dijken et al. reported that rats exposed to a single session of inescapable foot shock demonstrated enhanced anxiety-like behavior in the open field apparatus 21 days later (Van Dijken et al., 1992). Adamec and Shallow (1993) found that a single, 5-min exposure to a predator caused exaggerated anxiety-like behavior in the EPM in rats 14 days later. In contrast to the EPM or light–dark box tests, the marble-burying test measures fear of a novel, invasive object (De Boer and Koolhaas, 2003, Njung'e and Handley, 1991). Therefore, this study was performed in order to determine whether RR induces long-lasting changes in anxiety-like behavior in various tests during the post-stress period in addition to causing the long-lasting hyperreactivity of the HPA axis. Six days after the end of repeated restraint, we measured behavior of controls and of mice that had been exposed to repeated restraint in a defensive withdrawal apparatus but found no difference in anxiety-type behaviors. Previously we had detected an exaggeration of the corticosterone response to mild stress in rats previously exposed to repeated restraint stress (Harris et al., 2004), therefore we subjected half of each of the control and restrained mice in this study to a mild stress before testing behavior in the elevated plus maze. The results of this test, performed 9–10 days after the end of repeated restraint, indicated that mice that had been exposed to repeated restraint but not mild stress were more anxious than mice that had never been exposed to any stress. Therefore, we did not use mild stress in any of the two final behavioral tests of marble burying 17 days after the end of restraint or activity in a light–dark box 19–20 days after the end of restraint.

Section snippets

Methods

All animal procedures were approved by the University of Georgia Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and were in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.

Results

RR mice lost weight on the days of restraint (Fig. 1A; Stress: P < 0.00007, Day: P < 0.0000001). Following RR, RR mice gained weight at the same rate as controls, however, they had not returned to the weight of the controls 8 days after the end of restraint. RR mice exhibited a significant reduction in food intake only on the days of stress, with intake returning to pre-stress levels immediately after the end of restraint (Fig. 1B; RR: P < 0.002, Time: P < 0.0001, RR × Time: P < 0.001). The intake of the

Discussion

Previous studies have demonstrated that rats and mice exposed to RR show an exaggerated endocrine response to a subsequent novel MS administered 12 or 14 days after RR (Harris et al., 2004). Although RR animals exhibit no change in the daily nadir of corticosterone release, exposure to a novel MS in the post-stress period evokes an elevated release of corticosterone. The objective of this study was to determine whether RR induces long-lasting changes in other aspects of the stress response,

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Jessica Davenport for her help with behavior testing. This work was supported by National Institutes of Health grant: MH06828101 (R.B.S.H.).

References (39)

Cited by (48)

  • Acute stress increases behaviors that optimize safety and decreases the exploration of aversive areas

    2022, Learning and Motivation
    Citation Excerpt :

    However, the literature on the effects of restraint stress on exploration in animal models of anxiety is limited. Most works evaluate the late effect of restraint stress, e.g., a day after the restraint exposure (Mendonça-Netto & Guimarães, 1996; Mendonça & Guimarães, 1998; Padovan et al., 2000), or in the case of the L/D box, most studies are focused on the consequences of chronic stress (Chotiwat & Harris, 2006; Li et al., 2016; Naert et al., 2011). In addition, a handful of the behavioral studies made using restrain stress had the objective of evaluating its effect on different stages of memory (Bowman et al., 2003; Li et al., 2012; Troncoso et al., 2010).

  • Deficiency of the immunoproteasome subunit β5i/LMP7 supports the anxiogenic effects of mild stress and facilitates cued fear memory in mice

    2019, Brain, Behavior, and Immunity
    Citation Excerpt :

    Taken together, the results of these three behavioral paradigms do not argue for increased anxiety levels in β5i/LMP7-deficient mice. Anxiety levels in humans and animals can be increased by stress exposure (Chotiwat and Harris, 2006) and it is known that pro-inflammatory processes play a role in stress-increased anxiety (Diz-Chaves et al., 2013; Maes et al., 1998). Therefore, we exposed mice to mild foot shock stress and tested them 10 days later in the light–dark box.

  • Novel behavioral assays of spontaneous and precipitated THC withdrawal in mice

    2018, Drug and Alcohol Dependence
    Citation Excerpt :

    Repeated administration of THC desensitizes CB1 receptors (Sim-Selley, 2003), and genetic deletion of Cnr1 (which encodes the CB1 receptor) decreases struggling in the tail suspension test (Aso et al., 2008). In addition to their utility as drug screens, these behavioral tests are also stressors, because each induces corticosterone release (Aso et al., 2008; Chotiwat and Harris, 2006). In our present studies, separate groups of mice were repeatedly exposed to THC or the synthetic cannabinoid JWH-018, a prototypical component of first-generation synthetics, colloquially known as “Spice” sold in the US and Europe (Atwood et al., 2011).

View all citing articles on Scopus
View full text