Introduction

Variation in clinical practice is an important source of variance in health outcomes.1 The purpose of clinical guidelines is to improve patient outcomes by limiting inappropriate variation by outlining evidence-based practice (EBP).2,3 This evidence-based approach to care will have an increasing impact on everyday dental practice as more guidelines are introduced. However, it is well-documented (and lamented) that the publication of evidence relating to clinical practice, either as individual studies or as guidelines, does not automatically result in implementation by clinicians.4,5 The lag between the provision of evidence and its implementation by clinicians may be decades.

There have been over 240 controlled trials of interventions to influence the behaviour of health professionals. Implementation interventions tend to be aimed at increasing knowledge or skills and include approaches involving the dissemination of guidelines and educational materials, small group education, courses, and audit and feedback. However, systematic reviews of such interventions have shown that increasing knowledge and skills is usually insufficient to achieve changes in clinical behaviour.6,7,8,9,10 Yet, expensive implementation interventions continue to be developed and trialled using this unsuccessful paradigm. There is a need both for more effective methods of designing implementation interventions and for more efficient trial methods

Although implementing guidelines often requires clinicians to change their behaviour, there is little evidence that psychological models of behaviour change have been applied to the design of implementation interventions; yet, these models have been successfully used to predict variation in many different behaviours in many different populations.11,12 They provide a framework showing relationships between psychological variables such as beliefs, attitudes and intentions, and behaviour. These models have also been used to design interventions which have been successful in changing behaviour in many different populations.13 One aim of this study was to explore the feasibility of applying psychological models to the design of interventions relating to the implementation of EBP, which has yet to be determined.

In addition to enlightening the design of implementation interventions, psychological models may also inform implementation trial methodology. Currently the main means of testing the success of implementation interventions in different populations is in resource intensive service-level trials. Psychological theories model relationships between cognitive variables and behaviour. They therefore identify variables that are proximal predictors of behaviour. Thus, the likelihood of a successful trial may be considerably increased by first examining the effect of interventions on a proximal predictor of behaviour in a modelling experiment. It is reasonable to expect that an intervention which influences a proxy outcome will be more likely to influence behaviour in a full trial than an intervention which does not.

An example of a proximal predictor of behaviour is intention. While not everyone who intends to perform a behaviour will do so, research using psychological models (particularly the Theory of Planned Behaviour) provides ample evidence that intention to perform a behaviour is nevertheless one of the best predictors of actually performing it.14,15 It would be expected that an implementation intervention which successfully influences behavioural intention in a modelling experiment would be more likely to change evidence-based practice in a full trial than one which did not.

The Scottish Intercollegiate Guidelines Network (SIGN) has recently published evidence-based guidelines relating to the care and management of third molars.16 The guideline evidence supports the overall reduction of third molar extractions. An implementation intervention relating to the management of third molars would therefore be required to reduce this behaviour. Based on the psychology literature, the likelihood of a successful implementation trial would be increased if it employs an intervention that reduces dentists' intention to perform third molar extractions.

Research using psychological models provides guidance on designing an intention-behaviour intervention. Gollwitzer, Orbell, Sheeran and their colleagues have demonstrated that the likelihood of performing a behaviour can be increased by planning when you intend to perform it.17,18,19 Behavioural approaches point to the need to develop alternative behaviours as the most effective method of eliminating a behaviour.20 We therefore sought to reduce dentists' intention to extract third molars by having them plan alternative behaviours to extracting third molars.

Method

This was a randomized controlled trial. A preliminary power analysis suggested that a minimum sample of 102 dentists be recruited (across 3 groups: 1 intervention, 2 control subgroups) were required to detect an effect size of 0.40, alpha = 0.05, power = 0.95 (Faul & Erdfelder's (1992) Gpower program). Approximately 6 months following the postal distribution of guidelines on the management of third molars to all Scottish dentists, 205 dentists were randomly selected from the Scottish Dental Practice Board List and allocated to a control or intervention group using a random number generator from SPSS. Figure 1 illustrates the trial profile.

Figure 1
figure 1

Trial profile

Each group was mailed a questionnaire that asked participants to describe their background (post-graduate qualifications; number of years they have been in clinical practice) and their third molar-related experience (number of third molar patients seen in the previous year and month; number of third molar extractions personally performed in these periods). All questionnaires included a general knowledge quiz, derived from research findings relating to third molars. The quiz items covered a wide range of areas, answered on a 3-point scale (correct (score = 1), incorrect (score = 0), not sure (score = 0). Example items are: 'an asymptomatic third molar should not be removed when it is buried and in close relationship with the inferior dental nerve'; 'dentigerous cyst formation is rare in association with third molars'. Although background variables (demographic, third molar experience and general knowledge) were not expected to be influenced by the intervention, the information was collected to establish any baseline group differences, since these variables may possibly influence third molar management or the effectiveness of the intervention.

The main outcome measure was intention to extract third molars. This was measured with three questionnaire items. Two items were concerned with the dentists' intention to personally extract third molars: 'of all the patients you see in the next month who require a third molar extraction, approximately how many do you intend to perform?' answered on a 4-point scale (none / some / most / all); 'how likely is it that you will extract a third molar within the next month?' answered on a 7-point scale (unlikely/likely); and one item concerned with following the SIGN guidelines (which support doing less third molar extractions): 'do you intend to follow the third molar guidelines?' answered on a 7-point scale (do / do not). Since the items were answered on different scales, answers were converted to z scores to ensure equal weighting, and then summed to create a single intention total with higher scores reflecting greater intention to extract third molars.

Intervention

The intervention involved asking participants to develop an alternative behaviour plan using an open question: 'if a patient reports to you with third-molar related pain and swelling, what alternative treatments to extraction would you consider?' Participants allocated to the intervention group were sent a questionnaire that asked this item prior to the intention items. According to the psychology models, this would have the effect of bringing to mind possible methods of treating third molar problems other than extraction before the formulation of an intention to extract. Having alternative behaviours in mind should thereby inhibit this formulation.

In order to ascertain that groups were equivalent in their specific knowledge (ie management alternatives to third molar extraction), a random sample of participants in the control group were sent a questionnaire which put this item after the intention items and the rest of the control group were sent a questionnaire which did not have this item at all (subgroups A and B, respectively). The questionnaires for participants in all groups were identical except for the placement of this single item.

Results

Data were analysed using SPSSPC.21 Group differences were investigated using Chi-square, t-tests and ANOVA (GLM). Relationships between variables were examined using regression analyses.

Participants

Ninety-nine dentists agreed to participate in the study by returning the questionnaires: 70 males and 29 females, with mean age = 41.42 years (SD = 8.62 years). Nineteen had been qualified less than 10 years, 20/99 had been qualified between 11 and 15 years, 36/99 had been qualified between 16 and 24 years, and 23/99 had been qualified over 25 years. Nineteen had a post-graduate qualification. Participants had seen, on average, 19 third molar patients in the previous year (ranging from 0 to 120) and two patients in the previous month (ranging from 0 to 15), and personally performed 12 third molar extractions in the previous year (ranging from 0 to 75) and 1 in the previous month (ranging from 0 to 13). The mean score on the general third molar knowledge quiz was 65% (11 out of 17 items; ranging from 0/17 to 15/17) (Cronbach alpha = 0.43). For the intervention item, the mean number of treatment alternatives to third molar extraction was 3. The treatment options listed were: antibiotics (51 / 99), mouthwash (30 / 99), oral hygiene instruction (22 / 99), periodontal therapy (21 / 99), operculectomy (17 / 99), monitoring (13 / 99), removal of an opposing third molar (13 / 99), pain relief (11 / 99), grinding (10 / 99), restoration (3 / 99), and removal of a second molar (3 / 99).

In order to ascertain, as far as possible, the comparability of respondents and non-respondents, information about non-respondents was sought from the Scottish Dental Register. There were no significant differences between the respondents and non-respondents in either gender (χ2 (1, 206) = 0.06, p = 0.88) or years qualified (t (1, 196) = 1.44, p = 0.15).

We were also able to compare the background data of our participants with an independent random sample of Scottish dentists who participated in a study investigating the influence of audit and feedback and computer-assisted learning on third molar management.22 There were no significant differences (at p<0.05) between participants in this study and participants in the independent study in background variables (age: t (1, 140) = −0.63, p = 0.53; gender: χ2 (1, 150) = 1.64, p = 0.24; years qualified: t (1, 147) = −1.11, p = 0.27, or third molar-related experience (patients seen in the previous year: t (1, 127) = −1.29, p = 0.20; or third molar extractions performed in the previous year: t (1, 129) = −1.07, p = 0.29).

Equivalence of groups

There were no significant differences (at p<0.05) between the control subgroups in any variable (return rate, background, independent or dependent) and so the subgroups were combined into a single control group for all reported analyses (control group N=66, intervention group N=33).

There was no significant difference in number of returned questionnaires by group (proportion returned: intervention group = 0.485, control group = 0.481; z = 0.054 ie < 1.96). There were no significant differences between the intervention and control groups in any background variable (age: t (1, 89) = 1.63, p = 0.11; gender: χ2 (1, 98) = 2.38, p = 0.30; years qualified: t (1, 96) = 1.13, p = 0.26), third molar-related experience (patients seen last year: t (1, 88) = −0.06, p = 0.95; extractions performed last year: t (1, 87) = −0.29, p = 0.77; patients seen last month: t (1, 87) = 0.43, p = 0.67; extractions performed last month: t (1, 85) = −0.47, p = 0.64), general third molar-related knowledge (t (1, 97) = −1.06, p = 0.29), or in the number of alternative treatment options listed (t (1, 61) = −1.49, p = 0.15).

Effect of the intervention on intention

The intervention was successful in influencing intention to extract third molars. Dentists in the intervention group had significantly lower intention to extract than dentists in the control group: control group mean = 0.39 (SD = 1.99); intervention group mean = −0.78 (SD =1.89); mean difference = 1.17 (SE = 0.42); 95% confidence interval for the difference = 0.34 to 1.99.

Post hoc analyses

The measure of intention had quite low internal reliability (Cronbach alpha = 0.40). Post hoc analyses were performed, exploring the effect of the intervention on each of the three intention items. The pattern was the same for each item as for the overall measure, in that the intervention group scored lower than the control group on each intention item, although, the difference between the groups was significant (at p<0.05) for only one of the three items (see Table 1).

Table 1 Mean differences between the control and intervention groups for separate intention items with 95% confidence intervals (CI)

Although there were no significant differences between the control and intervention groups in regard to background factors, these factors may still influence the effects of the intervention on dentists' intention to extract third molars. A multiple linear regression analysis was performed to investigate this possibility. However, only group (intervention and control) significantly contributed to the regression equation (at p<.05 level) predicting intention to extract (See Table 2).

Table 2 Results of the multiple linear regression analysis examining the effect of potential confounding factors as well as intervention group on intention to extract third molars (TMs)

Discussion

Research using psychological models provides evidence that suggests that intention to perform a behaviour is the most reliable predictor of implementing that behaviour. Guideline implementation interventions that do not influence intention to implement EBP are therefore unlikely to influence clinical practice. In this trial we used psychological models to develop an intervention, which successfully influenced the intention of dentists to implement third molar EBP in the desired direction. As predicted by behavioural models, planning for alternative behaviours had the effect of reducing intention to perform a specific behaviour incompatible with EBP.

It should be particularly noted that our intention intervention did not add information, unlike other guideline implementation interventions. All participants had received the guidelines before taking part in the study. Participants in the intervention and the control groups were able to demonstrate an equal amount of general knowledge relating to EBP. They were also equally familiar with knowledge relating to possible clinical alternatives to extraction. It was therefore neither the existence of guidelines nor level of knowledge that influenced clinician's intention to implement EBP. This finding may help to explain the general lag in the implementation of EBP and the lack of success of interventions based on educational approaches. Information and knowledge per se are just not enough to motivate EBP.

A limitation of the present study was the return rate, which was just under 50%. There was no offer of recompense for participating in this study, which suggests that dentists who did respond were quite motivated. It is possible that a degree of motivation may be required for the success of this particular intervention. There also may be some concern over how representative the participants in the study were of dentists in Scotland. Nevertheless, there was no significant difference in the return rate between the intervention and control groups. There were also no significant differences in gender or years registered between respondents and non-respondents, or between the background of participants and an independent sample of Scottish dentists. There is therefore no reason to believe that the response rate or the background of our particular sample of dentists biased the results.

Conclusion

This study examined the effects of an implementation intervention in the form of modelling experiment. The results of this study demonstrate the effectiveness of a theoretically based intervention. The study also complements and extends current findings on implementation interventions in dental practice. It suggests interventions be tested on a proxy outcome for behaviour, derived from theoretical models, as a possible means of increasing the likelihood of success of service-level trials. While caution is warranted in making generalizations about the effect on EBP, the evidence suggests this intervention, which successfully influenced a proximal predictor of behaviour pertinent to dental EBP, would be worth investigating in a service-level trial to increase dental EBP.

Applying psychological models to the implementation of dental EBP does not mean ignoring the necessity of educating clinicians in prerequisite knowledge or skills. However, there is ample evidence showing that information transfer is simply not enough to implement changes in clinicians' behaviour. We therefore need to take advantage of research using psychological models specifically directed at behaviour change. These models offer a means of identifying possible target variables, both dependent and independent, for guideline implementation interventions. Designing interventions based on these models also means that the methodology relating to the intervention design can be replicated.

However, using psychological models requires a paradigm shift in guideline implementation studies. The implementation of EBP needs to be conceptualized as behaviour, rather than as ignorance or negligence. Basing implementation interventions on psychological models may be an effective way to bridge the gap between clinical guidelines and clinicians' behaviour.