INTRODUCTION

Patients with schizophrenia have a 20% shorter lifespan than the general population and a 50% increased risk of death from medical causes (Harris and Barraclough, 1998). Cardiovascular disease is the main contributor to medical morbidity and mortality in the general population, and patients with schizophrenia are 2.3 times more likely to die from cardiovascular disease than the general population (Osby et al, 2000). Key modifiable risk factors for cardiovascular disease (insulin resistance, smoking, obesity, hypercholesterolemia, hypertension, inactivity, and hyperglycemia) are more prevalent in patients with schizophrenia than in the general population (Allebeck, 1989; Allison et al, 1999; Dixon et al, 1999; Herran et al, 2000; Davidson et al, 2001; Davidson, 2002). Treatment with antipsychotic medications has been associated with increases in weight and other cardiometabolic risk factors (American Diabetes Association, 2004; Casey et al, 2004; Newcomer, 2005; Haupt, 2006). Weight gain during antipsychotic treatment consists primarily of adiposity (Zhang et al, 2004; Graham et al, 2005) and increased adiposity is an established risk factor for diabetes and cardiovascular disease in the general population (Peiris et al, 1989; Koh-Banerjee et al, 2004).

One way to understand the contribution of antipsychotic medications to the risk for developing diabetes and cardiovascular disease is to examine the relationship between adiposity and insulin sensitivity. Although antipsychotic-induced increases in adiposity are a key hypothesized mechanism for antipsychotic-associated increases in the risk of diabetes, reports of adiposity-independent effects of medications (Houseknecht et al, 2007) and lack of consistent correlation between weight change and plasma glucose in short-term clinical trials (Meyer, 2002; Lindenmayer et al, 2003; Simpson et al, 2004) have led to uncertainty regarding the importance of weight gain in contributing to changes in glucose metabolism during antipsychotic treatment. Specifically regarding the relationship of weight gain to the risk of diabetes, questions have been raised about the applicability of general population data to antipsychotic-treated patients with schizophrenia (Boehm et al, 2004). The hypothesis that increases in adiposity are a risk factor for diabetes in the general population but are not related to the risk of diabetes during antipsychotic treatment suggests that there may not be a significant relationship between adiposity and insulin sensitivity in antipsychotic-treated patients. Although this alternate hypothesis seems less likely given the established relationship between adiposity and insulin resistance in nonpsychiatric samples, and the increased prevalence of diabetes and cardiovascular disease in treated psychiatric populations, the relationship between adiposity and insulin sensitivity is readily testable in treated psychiatric patients.

The sensitivity of techniques used to detect variation in insulin sensitivity is an important consideration in study design and interpretation. Frequently sampled intravenous glucose tolerance tests (FSIVGTT) provide measures of insulin sensitivity and β cell function in a single 3-h session. The FSIVGTT is useful because of its greater sensitivity compared with the fasting measures, and its significantly lower cost and complexity than the gold-standard hyperinsulinemic euglycemic clamp procedure. The FSIVGTT is more complicated with respect to data collection and interpretation than fasting assessments or a standard oral glucose tolerance test, but can be useful in research settings. Intravenous rather than oral administration of glucose avoids the variability seen in glucose-absorption rates through the gastrointestinal tract. Insulin sensitivity, as assessed by FSIVGTT, is significantly related to adiposity in studies of healthy human samples (Welch et al, 1990; Mayer-Davis et al, 1997). Using body mass index (BMI) or computed tomography as indicators of adiposity, adiposity has been reported to account for 23% (Karter et al, 1996) to 45% (Macor et al, 1997) of the variance in insulin sensitivity as measured by FSIVGTT in nonpsychiatric samples.

This study aimed to test the relationship between adiposity, measured as BMI or waist circumference, and a FSIVGTT-derived measure of insulin sensitivity, hypothesizing a significant association, similar to what has been reported in untreated, nonpsychiatric healthy human and healthy animal subjects. BMI and waist circumference were used as indirect measurements of adiposity; these measures (1) are readily available clinically, with BMI especially offering high test–retest reliability (Reaven, 2005); (2) are well-correlated with direct measurements of adiposity (Janssen et al, 2002); and (3) have predictive validity as indicators of insulin sensitivity even in clinical trial sized samples (McLaughlin et al, 2003). The secondary aims of this study were (1) to evaluate the relationship between adiposity and acute insulin response to glucose loading and (2) to evaluate adiposity-independent effects of antipsychotic treatment and psychiatric diagnosis on insulin sensitivity and secretion in BMI-matched treatment groups. Whereas different antipsychotic medications can induce different amounts of change in body weight and adiposity during prospective treatment (American Diabetes Association, 2004; Newcomer, 2005), differences in adiposity that emerge during treatment can complicate the assessment of adiposity-independent effects, leading this study and others to match treatment groups for adiposity to better test for adiposity-independent medication effects.

METHODS

Subjects

Sixty-three nondiabetic patients with schizophrenia and 16 control subjects participated after giving written informed consent. Studies were approved by the Institutional Review Boards for Washington University School of Medicine (WUSM) and the Missouri Department of Mental Health. Chronically treated patients with DSM-IV schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, aged 18–60 years, were recruited for study participation. Patients were required to have been receiving treatment with olanzapine (n=24), risperidone (n=16), ziprasidone (n=11), or first-generation antipsychotics (n=12) (Table 1), with no greater than 25% changes in antipsychotic dose over the 3 months before study entry. Duration of prior treatment ranged from 24 to 3 months (21 subjects 24 months, 8 subjects 18–24 months, 10 subjects 12–18 months, 10 subjects 6–12 months, 14 subjects 3–6 months), with a similar distribution of treatment duration for each of the treated subject groups, and no difference in mean duration of exposure (F(3,33)=1.51, p=0.23). No medication changes of any kind were permitted for 2 weeks before, or during, the period of study participation. Patients were recruited through outpatient clinics associated with Washington University School of Medicine. Healthy control subjects (n=14) were recruited through advertising.

Table 1 Descriptive Statistics (Mean±SD, Frequency Distribution) for Treated Schizophrenia (n=63) and Untreated Healthy Control (n=16) Subjects

Antipsychotic treatment groups were matched for BMI and age, using a method similar to that used in a previous study from our group, (Newcomer et al, 2002) excluding more obese or more or less aged subjects that might otherwise have been consecutively enrolled into specific treatment groups to maintain group matching. Whereas patients tended to have higher BMI and older age in comparison to controls, BMI matching was maintained across all treated patient groups, and more broadly across all subject groups (including treated patients and untreated healthy controls) (main effect of group on BMI: F(4,72)=1.67, p=0.17). Subject groups were not ideally matched for age (main effect of group on age: F(4,72)=3.08, p=0.021), explained by a slightly younger untreated healthy control group, with the treated patient groups well-matched for age (F(3,59)=1.08, p=0.36).

All subjects underwent a medical screening and diagnostic evaluation, including the Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies (Nurnberger et al, 1994) (DIGS) and a review of available medical records, with a final research diagnosis made by a research psychiatrist using Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) algorithms. Subjects were additionally excluded for (1) Axis I disorders except schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, and substance abuse and/or dependence occurring more than 6 months before the study entry; (2) medical conditions that could confound metabolic assessments, including history of diabetes mellitus, recognized cardiovascular and respiratory conditions with hemodynamic compromise or hypoxia, malignancy, epilepsy, endocrine disease (excluding corrected, stable thyroid abnormalities), fever, dehydration, nausea, body weight less than 80% of ideal, pregnancy or high-dose estrogen therapy, narcotic, oral corticosteroid or spironolactone therapy, or sedative hypnotic withdrawal. Similarly, no subjects treated with glucose- or lipid-lowering medications were included for analysis. Subjects were excluded for type 1 and type 2 diabetes mellitus as determined by a random glucose of 200 and/or a fasting glucose of 140 mg/dl (World Health Organization, 1985) based on diagnostic criteria in use at the time of study initiation. All patients were additionally characterized using the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (Overall, 1974) (BPRS; 18-item, 1–7 scale).

Baseline clinical data as well as mean doses of the primary antipsychotic treatments for each treated patient group are listed in Table 1. Twelve of the 63 patients were receiving first-generation antipsychotics. Of these 12 patients, two were receiving an oral agent only (one haloperidol 10 mg/day, one thiothixene 5 mg/day), six were receiving haloperidol decanoate as their only antipsychotic treatment (mean mg/month=272.22±188.76), three were receiving fluphenazine decanoate as their only antipsychotic treatment (mean mg/month=52.78±41.11), and one was receiving 10 mg oral haloperidol/day plus 150 mg haloperidol decanoate every month. Mean antipsychotic dose for first-generation oral agents (in haloperidol equivalents) is listed in Table 1. Additionally, six patients in this group were receiving an anticholinergic medication (benztropine mesylate, mean daily mg±SD=2.67±1.03).

Twenty-four of the 63 patients were receiving olanzapine (Table 1). Of these 24, seven were receiving adjunctive first-generation antipsychotics (one receiving fluphenazine decanoate 50 mg every 2 weeks, one receiving trifluoperazine 10 mg/day, one receiving haloperidol 20 mg/day, four receiving haloperidol deconoate mean mg/month=183.33±102.74). Six patients were receiving benztropine mesylate, mean dose 2.5±1.22 mg/day. Three patients were receiving mood stabilizers: valproate 1500 mg/day, lamotrigine 450 mg/day, and gabapentin 1800 mg/day.

Sixteen of the 63 patients were receiving risperidone (Table 1). Of those 16, two were receiving anticholinergics (benztropine mesylate mean daily mg±SD=1.5±0.71). In addition, one patient was receiving adjunctive haloperidol decanoate 200 mg every 2–3 weeks, one was receiving lithium 600 mg/day, and one was receiving valproate 1500 mg/day.

Eleven of the 63 patients were receiving ziprasidone (Table 1). Of those 11, four were receiving anticholinergics (three benztropine mesylate mean daily mg±SD=2.83±1.89, one trihexyphenidyl 1 mg/day). In addition, one patient was receiving adjunctive haloperidol decanoate 100 mg/month, and two patients were receiving valproate 750 mg/day.

Of all 63 treated subjects, nine were receiving benzodiazepines, two were receiving zolpidem, seven were receiving antidepressants, eight were receiving antihypertensive medications, and one was receiving conjugated estrogens 0.625 mg/medroxyprogesterone 2.5 mg daily.

Procedure

This study used the well-characterized modified FSIVGTT procedure (Ader et al, 1985; Beard et al, 1986; Bergman et al, 1987; Bergman, 1989; Welch et al, 1990; Steil et al, 1993) including frequent plasma sampling and a glucose challenge followed by an insulin challenge (Welch et al, 1990) in combination with a computerized mathematical model (Bergman's minimal model, or MinMod), to generate measures of insulin secretion (AIRG) and insulin sensitivity (SI) (Matthews et al, 1985; Haffner et al, 1996; Hermans et al, 1999a, 1999b). Intravenous infusion of glucose is followed by intravenous infusion of insulin, with frequent blood sampling (details below). The resulting data are analyzed by MinMod, which assumes that (1) glucose inhibits its own production, and increases its own utilization proportional to its plasma concentration, (2) that insulin acts synergistically with glucose to promote these effects, and (3) the effects of insulin are proportional to its concentration in a compartment separate from plasma. The software uses these assumptions to adjust the values of the differential equations that describe the model until the recorded pattern of plasma glucose values is fitted (Bergman, 1989).

The homeostasis model assessment (HOMA) was used secondarily to calculate an index of insulin resistance (HOMA IR) using the formula HOMA IR=(fasting insulin (μU/ml) × fasting glucose (mmol/l))/22.5 (Matthews et al, 1985).

Subjects consumed a standardized diet providing 30 kcal/kg body weight/day (50% carbohydrate and 33% fat), provided to them by Washington University General Clinical Research Center (GCRC) dietary resources, for 3 days before each study day. There was a small but statistically significant difference in average daily calories consumed across subject groups (F(4,70)=2.85, p=0.03) ranging from 2033 kcal/day consumed by the ziprasidone group to 2488.5 kcal/day consumed by the control group (Table 2). However, when the diets were separated into carbohydrates, fat, and protein for more in-depth analyses, there were no significant differences between subject groups in the average daily consumption of any of the three categories (F(4,70)=0.38, p=0.82; F(4,70)=1.37, p=0.25; and F(4,70)=1.05, p=0.39; respectively).

Table 2 Dietary Composition (Mean±SD) for Treated Schizophrenia (n=61) and Untreated Healthy Control (n=14) Subjects

At 0800 on the study days, after an 8–10 h overnight fast (except for water), two intravenous lines were inserted, one for infusion and the other for blood sampling, by the supervising physician and GCRC nursing personnel who were present throughout the session. Subjects were reclining at a roughly 45° angle in bed, with one hand kept warm in a clear acrylic box thermostatically warmed to 50°C to arterialize the blood samples (obtained from Dr Dwight Matthews, University of Vermont, dmatthew@uvm.edu), which were drawn from an indwelling catheter in that hand or forearm. Infusions were made through the contralateral catheter. A bolus of regular human recombinant insulin (0.03 U/kg) was given at 20 min (Novo Nordisk Pharmaceuticals Inc., Princeton, NJ, USA). Blood samples to measure plasma glucose and insulin concentrations were drawn at −15, −10, −5, 2, 4, 8, 19, 22, 30, 40, 50, 60, 70, 80, 100, and 180 min after the start of the dextrose infusion. In addition, baseline (−15, −10, −5 min) preload fasting measures of C-peptide and glucagon were collected (an average of the three values was used for analysis). Our use of a modified FSIVGTT procedure with 16 sampling times was derived from previously reported FSIVGTT procedures with 12–33 sampling times (Bergman et al, 1981; Steil et al, 1993). MinMod is designed to accept a range of sample timepoints and sample frequencies in its calculation SI and AIRG. From both a physiologic and mathematical modeling perspective, the most critical issue in the derivation of SI is the accuracy and value of plasma glucose and insulin immediately following the 20-min exogenous insulin infusion. The number and frequency of samples at timepoints more distal to the insulin infusion have little effect on MinMod calculations. Our protocol allowed high-density sampling at the critical baseline and post-glucose and post-insulin infusion periods, with less frequent sampling at the end of the study where fewer data are needed to construct an accurate curve. A urine drug screen was administered to subjects with any relatively recent medical record documentation of substance abuse to confirm no active use.

Waist circumference was determined by placing a tape measure around the abdomen at the level of the iliac crest snugly without causing skin compression, parallel to the floor at the end of normal expiration (National Institutes of Health, 2000). BMI was calculated as the weight in kilograms divided by the square of the height in meters (National Institutes of Health, 2000).

Data Analysis

Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was used to test the hypotheses concerning main and interactive effects of adiposity (BMI or waist circumference) and subject group (group factor with five levels: risperidone, olanzapine, ziprasidone, first-generation antipsychotic, and control) on insulin sensitivity (MinMod SI). Secondary analyses tested these same effects on pancreatic β-cell function (MinMod AIRG). Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was further used to test the secondary hypothesis concerning any main effect of subject group (group factor with five levels: risperidone, olanzapine, ziprasidone, first-generation antipsychotic, and control) on insulin sensitivity (MinMod SI). The overall significance level was set at p=0.05.

To ensure group comparability, ANOVA was used to test for an effect of subject group on either BMI or age. In addition to effects of BMI (Resnick et al, 1998) and age (Harris et al, 1998), variables such as ethnicity and smoking may also be associated with differences in insulin sensitivity (Resnick et al, 1998). As an additional precaution against confounders to the interpretation of the results, each of these variables was individually added as either a covariate term (ANCOVA) or factor to the main models.

RESULTS

Adiposity significantly predicted insulin sensitivity in treated patients and untreated healthy controls. Including all subject groups in the model (five groups: risperidone, olanzapine, typicals, ziprasidone, and untreated healthy controls) and adiposity as the covariate term (using BMI or waist circumference, respectively) in an ANCOVA model predicting insulin sensitivity, a main effect of adiposity was detected for MinMod SI (F(1,62)=35.11, p<0.0001 and F(1,46)=24.48, p<0.0001, respectively, see Figure 1). Consistent with previous results from nonpsychiatric human samples, BMI or waist circumference explained a third of the variance in SI in this sample. Similarly, adiposity significantly predicted the acute insulin secretory response to glucose challenge (MinMod AIRG) in treated patients and untreated healthy controls. In a similar ANCOVA model, a main effect of adiposity (using BMI or waist circumference, respectively) was detected for MinMod AIRG (F(1,65)=22.42, p<0.0001 and F(1,49)=11.72, p=0.0013, respectively, see Figure 2).

Figure 1
figure 1

Insulin sensitivity (SI) calculated using MiniMod in healthy controls and treated schizophrenia patients as a function of BMI (R2=0.338) and waist circumference (R2=0.331), with regression lines and 95% confidence intervals displayed.

Figure 2
figure 2

Acute insulin response (AIRG) to intravenous glucose administration in healthy controls and treated schizophrenia patients as a function of BMI (R2=0.229) and waist circumference (R2=0.165), with regression lines and 95% confidence intervals displayed.

There were no consistent effects of medication group on either insulin sensitivity or secretion, independent of adiposity. This was indicated by the detection of no significant main effects or interactions of subject group on MinMod SI (F(4,46)=2.32, p=0.07) or MinMod AIRG (F(4,49)=0.23, p=0.92) in these ANCOVA models using waist circumference as an indicator of adiposity. Using BMI as the indicator of adiposity in those models, there were no interactions with subject group on either MinMod AIRG or MinMod SI and no main effect of subject group on MinMod AIRG (F(4,65)=0.23, p=0.92). There was a main effect of subject group on MinMod SI using BMI as the indicator of adiposity (F(4,62)=2.57, p=0.05), but Bonferroni–Dunn post hoc comparisons detected no significant differences in SI between any two individual subject groups.

In general, adiposity was also a predictor of measured values for other indices of glucose and lipid metabolism. A significant main effect of BMI or waist circumference, with no significant main effect of, or interaction with, subject group was detected on several variables. A main effect of BMI was detected for fasting triglyceride (F(1,65)=4.18, p=0.045), average fasting insulin (F(1,66)=44.04, p<0.0001), HOMA IR (F(1,66)=37.16, p<0.0001), and HOMA β cell function (F(1,66)=39.96, p<0.0001). A main effect of waist circumference was detected for average fasting insulin (F(1,50)=34.64, p<0.0001), HOMA IR (F(1,50)=30.12, p<0.0001), and HOMA β cell function (F(1,50)=31.99, p<0.0001). No significant effect of BMI or waist circumference on fasting plasma glucose was detected.

Exploratory analyses of other variables reported to be related to adiposity in nonpsychiatric populations also showed an effect of adiposity in treated subjects. A significant main effect of BMI on total cholesterol (F(1,67)=4.56, p=0.037), average baseline C-peptide (F(1,67)=39.26, p<0.0001), and fasting glucagon (F(1,67)=7.19, p=0.0092) was detected, with no main effect of or interaction with subject group. A main effect of waist circumference on systolic blood pressure (BP) (F(1,51)=4.50, p=0.039), diastolic BP (F(1,51)=5.00, p=0.0298), average baseline C-peptide (F(1,51)=40.96, p<0.0001), and fasting glucagon (F(1,51)=6.20, p=0.0161) was detected, with no main effect of or interaction with subject group. There was a significant interaction between waist circumference and subject group in the prediction of diastolic BP (F(4,51)=2.58, p=0.0482).

The primary finding that adiposity, but not disease or medication condition, significantly contributes to the variance in insulin sensitivity and secretion was not altered by including in the model factors such as ethnicity, smoking status, or by including or excluding the subjects receiving concomitant medications, all of which might be hypothesized to confound the interpretation of results. In an ANCOVA model incorporating smoking status (ie current packs per day) and ethnicity, respectively, the main effect of BMI on MinMod SI remained significant (F(1,52)=7.71, p=0.0076 and F(1,51)=24.61, p=0.0001, respectively), with no main effect of subject group (F(4,52)=0.66, p=0.62 and F(4,51)=1.63, p=0.182, respectively), no main effect of smoking status (F(1,52)=0.81, p=0.37) or ethnicity (F(1,51)=3.31, p=0.075), and no significant two- or three-way interactions involving BMI, subject group, and either ethnicity or smoking status. Similar results were found for MinMod AIRG.

Comments

The primary result of this study using MinMod analysis of FSIVGTT data is that BMI and waist circumference significantly predict insulin sensitivity in antipsychotic-treated patients with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder as well as in untreated healthy controls. These results extend previous studies that describe the association between adiposity and insulin sensitivity in nonpsychiatric subjects, indicating that this association is similarly observed in patients with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder treated with antipsychotic medications. The amount of variance in insulin sensitivity (33–34%) that was explained by adiposity in this study is consistent with previous results from a variety of nonpsychiatric human subject samples; these results do not support the perspective that the relationship between adiposity and insulin sensitivity is fundamentally different in patients treated with antipsychotics as compared with untreated humans without psychiatric disease. Whereas patients’ level of adiposity can certainly be a function of medication effects, additional nonadiposity-related medication effects or schizophrenia-related effects might add to or interact with the large effect of adiposity on these parameters. However, this study did not detect significant adiposity-independent effects (ie effects of medication group or disease group) on insulin sensitivity, or on the initial insulin response to glucose. These results strongly suggest that the effect of adiposity on insulin sensitivity in patients receiving antipsychotic treatment is relatively large as compared with adiposity-independent effects.

Some previous studies in antipsychotic-treated psychiatric patients have conducted post hoc or exploratory examinations of the relationship of weight or adiposity to measures of glucose metabolism, sometimes failing to detect a significant relationship between weight change and some indication of altered metabolism (Meyer, 2002; Lindenmayer et al, 2003; Simpson et al, 2004). Despite the limitations of some of these analyses, the results have prompted questions about the association between antipsychotic-related changes in weight or adiposity and risk for disturbances in glucose metabolism (Boehm et al, 2004). Failure to detect significant correlations involving weight and glucose metabolism in some previous studies may, in part, be related to the common use of fasting plasma glucose as the indicator of glucose metabolism. Changes in fasting plasma glucose in response to increasing insulin resistance are initially buffered by compensatory increases in insulin secretion that prevent hyperglycemia as long as pancreatic β cell reserve remains available. Therefore, studies of relatively short duration in patients who still have reserve insulin secretory capacity would not be expected to observe changes in fasting plasma glucose despite adiposity-related increases in insulin resistance, whereas patient samples with limited β cell reserve (eg, older subjects with an extended prior duration of insulin resistance) might manifest increases in fasting plasma glucose in relation to increased insulin resistance within short time frames. The current sample of patients is likely to be representative of many published schizophrenia samples. In the current study, a significant correlation was observed between adiposity and pancreatic β cell insulin secretory response to glucose loading (measured as AIRG), suggesting some degree of intact β cell response to insulin resistance. Consistent with this, the current study found no correlation between adiposity and fasting plasma glucose. The use of a validated measure of insulin sensitivity in this study, instead of fasting glucose, allowed the demonstration of an important effect of adiposity on glucose metabolism. It should be noted that had this study used only fasting plasma glucose as a measure of glucose metabolism, it would have been unable to detect the deleterious effect of increasing adiposity.

The current study did not provide evidence that adiposity-independent effects of medications or psychiatric disease state make a significant contribution to insulin sensitivity or the acute insulin response to glucose. No significant effect of treatment group or interaction with treatment group (olanzapine, risperidone, ziprasidone, first-generation antipsychotics, and untreated healthy controls) on insulin sensitivity or acute insulin response was detected. It should be noted that the failure to detect an effect of treatment group on insulin sensitivity may be a type II error, and studies with similar sample size have, previously, detected such treatment group effects (Newcomer et al, 2002; Henderson et al, 2005). However, the ‘adiposity-independent’ effects of treatment group previously reported in these studies were detected when subjects were similarly matched for adiposity using BMI, which in some individuals or cohorts might fail to account for treatment group-related differences in intramuscular or abdominal fat; in this way, treatment group effects detected in those studies may actually be accounted for by treatment group-related differences in adiposity. In addition, unlike other studies, this study provided a dietary control for all subjects, reducing treatment group-related differences in total calorie and fat intake. This procedure may have obscured medication-related differences in food preference and calorie intake that might have contributed to the treatment group differences in insulin sensitivity observed in other studies. In any case, direct effects of antipsychotic medications on insulin sensitivity—ie those not mediated by adiposity—may be relatively small and more difficult to experimentally detect in comparison to the larger effects of adiposity on insulin sensitivity.

The current findings are relevant to the prediction of risk for adverse health outcomes related to decreased insulin sensitivity. Decreases in insulin sensitivity are an important early event in the development of the insulin resistance syndrome (Reaven, 2005) and the metabolic syndrome (National Cholesterol Education Program, 2002), which are major risk factors for cardiovascular disease and type 2 diabetes mellitus. The predictive effect of adiposity in this study was found across all treatment conditions, including untreated healthy controls and all medication groups. The strong relationship between adiposity and insulin sensitivity is consistent with numerous prior reports of intravenous glucose tolerance testing in nonpsychiatric populations, (Welch et al, 1990; Karter et al, 1996; Macor et al, 1997; Mayer-Davis et al, 1997) and consistent with an emerging consensus regarding the importance of increased adiposity in psychiatric populations.

There were limitations to this study. Owing to the interest in matching for adiposity, which is not possible during prospective treatment with medications that have differential effects in this area, this study was not a prospective, randomized trial. Such trials including detailed measures of insulin sensitivity are underway, and will be useful for quantifying treatment-induced changes in adiposity and related treatment-induced changes in insulin sensitivity. The use of chronically treated, stable outpatients in this study, rather than more acutely ill inpatients, may not capture early treatment-induced changes in insulin sensitivity that could occur in advance of changes in adiposity (Houseknecht et al, 2007). However, the use of stable outpatients provides the benefit of minimizing potentially confounding metabolic effects of acute psychological stress, most notably acute elevations in plasma cortisol, which have been shown to affect insulin sensitivity and pancreatic β cell function (Bjorntorp and Rosmond, 2000). On a related note, studies of acutely ill psychiatric populations may overestimate insulin resistance that might be attributable to schizophrenia in general, owing to these acute metabolic effects of stress (Ryan et al, 2003; Reynolds, 2006). This study also contained subjects taking concomitant medications, which have the potential to obscure the effects of individual antipsychotic medications. However, the significance of the primary result that BMI strongly predicts MinMod SI was not changed when subjects taking different concomitant medications were serially removed from the analysis. This study used BMI or waist circumference as an indicator of adiposity, rather than a direct measure of whole-body or regional adiposity that could provide better sensitivity to detect effects of interest. It should be noted, however, that BMI has well-established predictive validity for insulin sensitivity (Karter et al, 1996), and is a readily available measure in clinical practice with high test-retest reliability.

Although the results of this study indicate that adiposity is a significant predictor of insulin sensitivity, adiposity alone does not predict all the variance. In this study, adiposity as estimated by BMI or waist circumference explained on average approximately 33–34% of the variance in insulin sensitivity (Figure 1). Studies using FSIVGTTs in nonpsychiatric samples to determine the predictive value of BMI on insulin sensitivity, have reported that BMI explains from 23% (Karter et al, 1996) to 45% (Macor et al, 1997) of the variance in insulin sensitivity. Additional contributions to variance in insulin sensitivity may come from genotype (Valle et al, 1998; Lebovitz, 1999; Watanabe et al, 1999; Elbein et al, 2000; Baroni et al, 2001; McIntyre and Walker, 2002) and fitness level (Reaven, 2003).

In summary, this study confirms that adiposity is a significant predictor of insulin sensitivity during antipsychotic treatment. A decrease in insulin sensitivity is a key event in the development of physiological changes associated with the insulin resistance syndrome and the metabolic syndrome, leading to increased risk for cardiovascular disease and type 2 diabetes mellitus. Increases in adiposity that occur as a result of antipsychotic treatment should be anticipated to potentially increase insulin resistance and cardiometabolic risk as much as other causes of increased adiposity. The predictive effect of adiposity on insulin sensitivity can be readily observed using the clinically available measures of BMI and waist circumference, supporting the value of these measures in recent guidelines concerning risk management during antipsychotic therapy (American Diabetes Association, 2004).