The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Published Online:

Abstract

Objective: Anatomic magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies have detected smaller cerebellar volumes in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) than in comparison subjects. However, the regional specificity and longitudinal progression of these differences remain to be determined. The authors compared the volumes of each lobe of the cerebellar hemispheres and vermis in children with ADHD and comparison subjects and used a new regional cerebellar volume measurement to characterize the developmental trajectory of these differences. Method: In a longitudinal case-control study, 36 children with ADHD were divided into a group of 18 with better outcomes and a group of 18 with worse outcomes and were compared with 36 matched healthy comparison subjects. The volumes of six cerebellar hemispheric lobes, the central white matter, and three vermal subdivisions were determined from MR images acquired at baseline and two or more follow-up scans conducted at 2-year intervals. A measure of global clinical outcome and DSM-IV criteria were used to define clinical outcome. Results: In the ADHD groups, a nonprogressive loss of volume was observed in the superior cerebellar vermis; the volume loss persisted regardless of clinical outcome. ADHD subjects with a worse clinical outcome exhibited a downward trajectory in volumes of the right and left inferior-posterior cerebellar lobes, which became progressively smaller during adolescence relative to both comparison subjects and ADHD subjects with a better outcome. Conclusions: Decreased volume of the superior cerebellar vermis appears to represent an important substrate of the fixed, nonprogressive anatomical changes that underlie ADHD. The cerebellar hemispheres constitute a more plastic, state-specific marker that may prove to be a target for clinical intervention.

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a developmental disorder affecting 5%–10% of school-age children and 4.4% of adults (1) . Clinical features include inattentiveness, motor hyperactivity, and impulsivity. Although considerable progress has been made in defining the neural substrate for the disorder, a paucity of longitudinal studies has hindered the development of a unified hypothesis to explain the clinical features of the disorder.

There has been increasing interest in the role of the cerebellum in the pathogenesis of ADHD. Structural anomalies of the cerebellum are among the most consistently reported features of ADHD, notably reductions in overall volume (2) , particularly in the cerebellar vermis (37) . Diffusion tensor imaging of cerebellar white matter has revealed that attentional impairment is associated with decreased fractional anisotropy in this region (8) . Although the cerebellum has traditionally been considered a site of motor control, lesion studies and functional imaging studies in healthy subjects have demonstrated a wide range of cognitive and affective functions in cerebellar structures, including temporal information processing, shifting attention, verbal working memory, implicit learning, executive function, and emotional regulation (912) . Dysfunction in each of these cognitive domains has in turn been implicated in the etiology of ADHD (13 , 14) .

Measurement of the cerebellum is technically difficult, complicated by differences in nomenclature, poorly defined gray-white boundaries, and a reliance on two-dimensional measurement (15) . Previous studies have been largely cross-sectional and thus have been unable to test the hypothesis that differences in the trajectory of growth rather than differences in absolute volume at single time point may be the most useful neuroanatomic endophenotype for ADHD. Our previous longitudinal study (2) suggested a fixed, nonprogressive reduction in total cerebellar volume but did not examine the possibility that the deficits in ADHD are highly regional.

We reexamined longitudinal data from a sample of 36 children with ADHD and 36 children without ADHD, all of whom underwent neuroanatomic magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans on at least three separate occasions. The current study included 157 scans from the children for whom we previously reported changes in total cerebellar volume (2) and an additional 72 scans acquired since that time, for a total of 229 scans. We measured the three hemispheric lobes and the corpus medullare on each side as well as three lobes of the midline vermis. We hypothesized that we would replicate the finding of smaller overall cerebellar volumes reported in previous structural imaging studies, and we expected to refine previous findings of a smaller midsagittal area in the inferior vermis (37) through a novel method of volumetric analysis. Given evidence regarding regional cerebellar activation during attention tasks in functional studies (16) , we also anticipated prominent findings in the posterior hemispheres. By adopting a longitudinal design, we further aimed to characterize the developmental trajectories of individual hemispheric and vermal lobar volumes in relation to the diagnosis of ADHD and in relation to long-term clinical outcome. In light of recent findings relating clinical outcome to abnormalities in the prefrontal cortex (17) , we hypothesized that volume normalizes in some regions of the cerebellum as disease symptoms diminish, thus distinguishing groups with better and worse outcomes.

Method

Participants

Thirty-six children and adolescents who met DSM-IV criteria for ADHD and for whom at least three usable MRI scans were available were selected from a larger sample of participants recruited for imaging studies at the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH). Recruitment details have been described elsewhere (2) . Diagnoses were made on the basis of the Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents (18) and a rating on the hyperactivity subscale of the Conners’s Teacher Rating Scale greater than two standard deviations above age- and sex-specific mean ratings (19) . Exclusion criteria were a full-scale IQ score below 80 and evidence of medical or neurological disorders. Comorbid psychiatric diagnoses among participants were relatively mild and were not the focus of treatment in any case. Thirty-five participants had combined-type ADHD (97%), and one (3%) had the hyperactive subtype. Fourteen participants (39%) underwent four scans, and one (3%) underwent five scans.

Thirty-six unrelated comparison subjects who had no personal or family history of psychiatric or neurological disorders were recruited from the community as part of the ongoing recruitment of the Child Psychiatry Branch at NIMH. Matched selection was used to minimize group differences in IQ, socioeconomic status, gender, and handedness. All subjects had undergone at least three scans; 18 (50%) had four scans, four (11%) had five scans, and one (3%) had seven scans.

The NIMH institutional review board approved the research protocol, and written informed consent and assent to participate in the study were obtained from the parents and children, respectively.

Clinical Outcome Measures

The last available score on the Children’s Global Assessment Scale (CGAS) (20) was used to characterize clinical outcome. Patients with ADHD were divided into groups with better (N=18) and worse (N=18) outcomes on the basis of the mean final follow-up CGAS score, which was 62.4 (SD=14.6); a better outcome was defined as a CGAS score ≥62 and a worse outcome as a CGAS score <62. CGAS scores, symptom scores, and diagnostic data were available for all ADHD patients at baseline and at the last follow-up. Clinical assessments were performed independently of neuroimaging analyses. The mean ages of the groups at each wave of the assessment and MRI scanning did not differ significantly. At the last follow-up, patients were further categorized according to DSM-IV criteria as having inattentive subtype only (N=11), hyperactive/impulsive subtype only (N=2), or combined type (N=12) or as having attained full remission (N=10).

Neuroanatomic Analyses

T1-weighted images with contiguous 1.5-mm sections in the axial plane and 2.0-mm sections in the coronal plane were obtained using three-dimensional spoiled gradient recalled echo in the steady state on a Signa 1.5-T scanner (General Electric Medical Systems, Milwaukee, Wisc.) (echo time=5 msec; repetition time=24 msec; flip angle=45°; acquisition matrix=256×192; number of signals acquired=1; field of view=24 cm). The original MR images were registered into standardized stereotaxic space using a linear transformation and corrected for nonuniformity artifacts (21) . An advanced neural net classifier was used to segment the registered and corrected volumes into white matter, gray matter, CSF, and background (22) .

Gray and white matter volumes of the total cerebrum were quantified using an automated technique developed at the Montreal Neurological Institute that combines voxel-intensity based tissue classification into gray matter, white matter, and CSF with a probabilistic atlas (23 , 24) . The cerebellum could not be classified owing to the complexity of its gray-white border.

The cerebellar parcellation method developed by Pierson and colleagues at the University of Iowa (15) was used in conjunction with the BRAINS2 software package (25) to generate outlines of each lobe of the cerebellar cortex and corpus medullare. Briefly, 31 landmarks were manually defined and used as the basis for the application of a neural net to generate surface masks of each of the cerebellar subregions. These were then reviewed and edited manually. Volumes of each subregion were measured and summed to provide the total cerebellar volume.

Masks of vermal subregions were manually traced. A midline guide trace in the sagittal plane marked the superior, inferior, anterior, and posterior limits of the vermis, and the axial plane was used to generate guide traces approximating the lateral borders. The vermis was defined in each coronal slice by manual tracing with reference to these guide traces. Regional subdivision was accomplished through placement of limiting boundaries in the sagittal plane, where the primary and prepyramidal fissures were readily apparent throughout the vermis.

The left and right cerebellar cortices were each divided into anterior, superior-posterior, and inferior-posterior lobes using the nomenclature of Larsell and Jansen (26) . The anterior lobe was composed of lobules I–V and delineated by the primary fissure. The superior-posterior lobe was defined to include lobules VI and VIIA-folium in the vermis region and lobule VI and crus I of VIIA in the hemispheres. It was separated from the inferior-posterior lobe by the horizontal fissure. The inferior-posterior lobe consisted of the lobules VIIA-tuber through X. The three hemispheric lobes consist predominantly of gray matter, although white matter branching off into the folia was included in the lobes. The left and right corpora medullare were also defined, including all of the central region, which appears as white matter. The MRI acquisition method used for this study was not able to visualize or separate out the deep nuclei within the corpus medullare. The vermis was divided into anterior, superior, and inferior regions. The primary fissure determined the posterior extent of the anterior lobe, and the prepyramidal fissure separated the superior from the inferior lobe. These regions are illustrated in Figure 1 .

Figure 1. Regional Anatomic Measures for the Cerebellum a

a Reprinted from Pierson et al. (15), with permission from Elsevier.

Intraclass correlations (ICCs) for all hemispheric regions and the corpus medullare were >0.80 for both intrarater and interrater reliability. In the vermis, intrarater reliability ICCs were all >0.80, and interrater ICCs were all >0.68.

Statistical Analysis

Differences between groups were assessed by t tests and analysis of variance (ANOVA) for continuous measures and chi-square tests and Fisher’s exact test for categorical measures. Mixed-model polynomial regression was used to examine cerebellar development. This method was chosen over traditional methods, such as repeated-measures ANOVA, because it permits the inclusion of multiple measurements per person, missing data, and irregular intervals between measurements, thereby increasing statistical power. For the polynomial comparisons between ADHD and comparison groups, the i th individual’s j th cerebellar volume was modeled as follows:

Volume ij = intercept + β 1 (diagnosis=ADHD) + β 2 (age – mean age) + β 3 (age – mean age) 2 + β 4 (diagnosis=ADHD × [age – mean age]) + β 5 (diagnosis=ADHD × [age – mean age] 2 ) + di + eij

where the intercept and b terms are fixed effects, di is a normally distributed random effect that models within-person dependence, and eij represents the usual normally distributed residual error. F values determined whether polynomial terms significantly contributed to the explanatory power of a model. After the order of the model was determined (linear or quadratic), F or t tests were used to determine whether the diagnostic curves differed in shape (that is, whether the coefficient[s] for age terms differed between groups) and height (group difference in expected volume at the average age). Parameter estimates of the fixed effects were used to generate fitted values in the graphs. Similar analyses were conducted for the three group comparisons. Fitted lines spanned the middle 80% of the data.

In view of the multiple statistical tests conducted, we adopted an alpha threshold of 0.01. Cohen’s d was used to calculate effect sizes (27) .

Results

Demographic and Clinical Characteristics

The ADHD and comparison groups were statistically comparable on all characteristics except handedness ( Tables 1 and 2 ). Outcome groups, defined either by the final CGAS score or by DSM-IV diagnosis at follow-up, were statistically comparable on all clinical characteristics except frequency of baseline diagnoses of oppositional defiant disorder and anxiety disorders.

Neuroanatomic Results

ADHD Group Versus Comparison Group

In the time 1 comparisons, relative to the comparison group, the ADHD group had significantly smaller volumes of the superior vermis (p=0.0007; d=0.85) and whole vermis (p=0.006; d=0.68) ( Table 3 ). In the comparisons across the trajectory of development, all cerebellar regions were modeled using quadratic terms except the three vermal subdivisions, the whole vermis, and the corpora medullare, where a constant growth model was appropriate.

Consistent with the time 1 comparisons, diagnostic differences at the sample average age (13.57 years) were significant for the superior vermis (F=13.95, df=1, 155, p=0.0003) and the whole vermis (F=4.55, df=1, 155, p=0.01).

The ADHD and comparison groups differed significantly with respect to development of the left anterior hemisphere, with the trajectory of the ADHD group converging to that of the comparison group by late adolescence (difference in shape, F=7.13, df=2, 153, p=0.001) ( Figure 2 ). Developmental trajectories for all other cerebellar regions were statistically parallel, including the superior and whole vermis measures, with the ADHD group’s mean volume remaining smaller over the course of the age span.

Figure 2. Developmental Trajectory of Left Anterior Hemisphere in ADHD Patients and Healthy Comparison Subjects a

a Significant difference between ADHD group (115 scans) and healthy comparison group (114 scans) (F=7.17, df=2, 153, p=0.001).

Better Versus Worse Clinical Outcome ADHD Groups

In the time 1 comparisons of the better and worse outcome groups, differences were observed in the superior vermis (F=7.0, df=2, 69, p=0.002), with a Tukey post hoc test indicating significantly smaller volumes for the worse outcome ADHD group relative to the comparison group (Tukey’s honestly significant difference=5.04, df=2, 69, p=0.002) and nonsignificant differences for the remaining two comparisons ( Table 4 ).

In the comparisons across the trajectory of development, similar to the time 1 comparisons, the better and worse outcome groups had significantly smaller superior vermis volumes than the comparison group at the sample average age (13.57 years) (F=4.62, df=1, 154, p=0.01 and F=11.97, df=1, 154, p=0.0007, respectively).

The outcome groups differed in the trajectory of growth for the entire cerebellum (p=0.01). While the better outcome group exhibited a trajectory parallel to that of the comparison group, the worse outcome group exhibited a progressive relative decrease in total volume, falling further away from the normal trajectory during adolescence ( Figure 3 ). This pattern was largely attributable to bilateral changes in the inferior-posterior hemispheres of the worse outcome group (comparison with better outcome group, F=5.96, df=2, 151, p=0.003 and F=4.91, df=2, 151, p=0.01 for left and right, respectively; compared with the comparison group, F=5.03, df=2, 151, p=0.008 and F=5.3, df=2, 151, p=0.01, for left and right, respectively) ( Figure 4 ).

Figure 3. Developmental Trajectory of Whole Cerebellum in ADHD Patients With Better and Worse Outcomes and Healthy Comparison Subjects a

a Difference between better outcome group and worse outcome group, p=0.01; between worse outcome group and healthy comparison group, p=0.01; between better outcome group and healthy comparison group, n.s.

Figure 4. Developmental Trajectory of Left and Right Inferior-Posterior Hemispheres in ADHD Patients With Better and Worse Outcomes and Healthy Comparison Subjects a

a For the left inferior-posterior hemisphere, difference between better outcome group and worse outcome, p=0.003; between worse outcome group and healthy comparison group, p=0.008; between better outcome group and healthy comparison group, n.s. For the right inferior-posterior hemisphere, difference between better outcome group and worse outcome group, p=0.01; between worse outcome group and healthy comparison group, p=0.01; between better outcome group and healthy comparison group, n.s.

Differences in developmental trajectories were also noted in the left anterior hemisphere, which, as in the diagnostic comparison, demonstrated a pattern of normalization with age. This normalization was prominent in the better outcome group (F=7.42, df=2, 151, p=0.001) but did not reach statistical significance in the worse outcome group ( Figure 5 ).

Figure 5. Developmental Trajectory of Left Anterior Hemisphere in ADHD Patients With Better and Worse Outcomes and Healthy Comparison Subjects a

a Difference between better outcome group and worse outcome group, n.s.; between worse outcome group and healthy comparison group, p=0.07; between better outcome group and healthy comparison group, p=0.001.

To determine whether grouping the ADHD participants according to follow-up DSM-IV diagnosis would yield similar developmental patterns for the hemispheric volumes, we conducted post hoc mixed-effect regression analyses. Consistent with the above results, for the right inferior-posterior volumes, participants with persistent combined-type ADHD significantly diverged from the comparison group in a manner similar to that of the worse outcome group (F=4.32, df=2, 154, p=0.015). No other comparisons were statistically significant at ≤0.01 (see the figure in the data supplement that accompanies the online version of this article).

Discussion

This is the first structural imaging study of ADHD to delineate the development of each lobe of the cerebellar hemispheres using longitudinal data and a volumetric regional measure of the cerebellar vermis. Using both a new semiautomated method for analyzing hemispheric lobes and a new manual method for accurately quantifying vermal subdivisions, we detected a highly specific abnormality—a smaller superior vermis—that has not previously been detected using whole cerebellar volumes or midsagittal vermal areas. Furthermore, the longitudinal design and clinical outcome stratification allowed us to demonstrate hemispheric differences between better and worse outcome groups, with the worse outcome group deviating progressively with age. These findings emphasize the mixture of fixed and progressive neuroanatomic deficits in ADHD (2 , 17) . The former may reflect a fundamental disease phenotype, and the latter may represent plastic anatomic changes that are related to symptom status.

In addition to its role in motor control, the cerebellum contributes to a wide range of cognitive and affective processing. Lesion studies demonstrate critical roles for the cerebellum in motor and perceptual tasks in which events span milliseconds, thus requiring exquisite temporal control (28) , in the orientation of spatial attention (29 , 30) , in verbal working memory (31) , in language processing, and in affective regulation (10 , 32) . Functional imaging studies with healthy subjects similarly demonstrate cerebellar activation in wide range of cognitive tasks (12 , 33 , 34) . Precise localization of function within the cerebellum has proved challenging given the limits of functional imaging in the posterior fossa, the heterogeneity of cerebellar damage among patients with lesions, and the possibility that cognitive functions, such as verbal working memory, require the coordinated action of multiple cerebellar regions (31) . While this complicates interpretation of our more regional findings, some considerations are relevant.

The nonprogressive deficit localized in the superior vermis in ADHD patients in our study may represent a neuroanatomic basis of fundamental deficits in cognitive and affective processing that are resistant to plastic developmental changes in ADHD. Lesion studies have found additionally that the degree of hypoplasia in the cerebellar superior vermis is selectively associated with the severity of deficits in attention-orientation (30) . Similarly, in studies of superficial siderosis (10 , 35) —an acquired neurological disorder of hemosiderin deposition particularly affecting the superior cerebellar vermis—deficits were identified in speech production, visual recall, executive impairments, and social comprehension; Schmahmann (10) emphasized the latter in his clinical description of socioemotional dysregulation among patients with vermal damage. Functional imaging studies also implicate the vermis in complex cognitive activities, including time estimation (36) , perceptual processing of stimuli at short intervals (37 , 38) , and the alerting and executive control aspects of attention (39) . Deficits in time information are consistently found in ADHD, including problems with motor timing, time discrimination, and time reproduction (4044) . These deficits may lead to failures in appreciating the temporal structure of the environment, which could underpin many symptoms of ADHD (14) . For example, impulsive behaviors in a child with ADHD may represent a failure to produce the precise timing required to participate in social exchanges. Similarly, transient lapses in attention and response variability (noted most clearly in neuropsychological testing) may also reflect an impaired, inconsistent style of temporal information processing (13) .

In our study, participants with ADHD in the worse clinical outcome group showed a progressively smaller total cerebellar volume over time, attributable mainly to the deviant trajectory of the inferior-posterior hemispheres. Lesion and functional imaging studies converge to show the importance of the lateral cerebellar hemispheres in executive function (10 , 45) , spatial cognition, linguistic processing (32) , and verbal working memory (31 , 46 , 47) . Meta-analysis has confirmed moderate deficits in executive functions in ADHD (particularly of response inhibition, set shifting, and working memory) (48) , which are likely to represent one of possibly multiple pathways to the cardinal symptoms of the disorder (13) . For example, working memory deficits lead to lapses in the maintenance of task-relevant information “online” and could underlie some symptoms of inattention. Additionally, deficits in the cerebellar components of motor control and response inhibition may contribute to hyperactivity. Future work can assess whether the progressively deviant structural trajectory in those with worse clinical outcome in ADHD is accompanied by a pattern of progressive cognitive deficits that contribute to the persistence of symptoms.

As the study participants with ADHD moved into adolescence, the volume of the left anterior hemisphere converged to that of the comparison group. This volumetric normalization occurred in a region that some lesion studies have linked specifically with time perception and precise motor timing (9) . Recent high-resolution functional imaging studies with healthy subjects have demonstrated marked activation of the anterior and superior hemispheres (lobules V–VI) during the perception and motor performance of temporal sequences (49) and implicated the anterior and superior cerebellum in visual and memory search (50) . In adults with persistent ADHD, an anomalous decrease in activation of the superior and anterior hemispheres during working memory tasks directly implicates this region in one of the deficits associated with ADHD (51) . It is possible that the structural normalization we report in our better outcome group is associated with improvements in these and related cognitive functions, representing a possible plastic compensatory response.

No firm conclusions can be drawn about etiological factors in this descriptive study. Structural features of the cerebellum are highly heritable (with an additive genetic value of 0.49)—although less so than those of other lobes—and thus genetic factors are likely to be important (52) . It is also possible that the different trajectories within ADHD are determined by polymorphisms in monoaminergic neurotransmitter genes that are expressed in the cerebellum, such as polymorphisms of the dopamine D 4 receptor gene, which have been linked with clinical outcome (53) . The rates of oppositional defiant disorder and anxiety disorders were higher in the worse outcome group, which raises the possibility that these comorbid disorders partly account for the deviant developmental pathways. This is of particular interest given recent suggestions that the presence of conduct disorder and possibly oppositional defiant disorder defines a phenotypic subtype of ADHD (54) . However, in all our study subjects at baseline, the clinical picture was dominated by ADHD, and the comorbid conditions were not the focus of treatment. This question will be clarified in future studies with an expanded sample that includes a larger proportion of children with a worse outcome and no comorbid disorders.

Limitations of this study include problems inherent to use of a manual region-of-interest method of measuring the vermis, which is reflected in the ICCs. However, all interrater ICCs were within the range considered good, and all intrarater ICCs were excellent (greater than 0.8) (55) . Our sample consisted almost entirely of patients with combined-type ADHD, which limits the examination of subtypes of ADHD. While we did not find any statistical difference in whole cerebellar size in children with ADHD, the effect size reflecting the volumetric reduction in ADHD is in line with previous reports, which suggests that the study lacked the power to detect a statistically significant difference. However, by restricting our analysis to children for whom three or more scans were available, we gained the perspective of longitudinal growth trajectories, which refine and extend the volumetric differences noted in previous studies. A large proportion of our subjects were exposed to stimulant medication both prior to the first assessment and during the follow-up period, which complicates the interpretation of our findings because stimulants may have neurotrophic effects (2) . However, because the two ADHD groups had similar histories of exposure to stimulants at baseline, medication alone is unlikely to account for the differential trajectories linked with outcome. It is more difficult to evaluate systematically the possible trophic effects of other medications given the rarity of use in our sample.

In summary, we found a fixed, nonprogressive volume reduction in the superior cerebellar vermis of children with ADHD, which may prove to be a useful endophenotype in future genetic studies. In contrast, cerebellar hemispheres exhibit a trajectory of developmental growth that is state specific and may represent a potential target for clinical or pharmacological intervention in ADHD.

Received June 29, 2006; revisions received Aug. 30 and Oct. 4, 2006; accepted Oct. 12, 2006. From the Columbia University School of Medicine, New York, N.Y.; Child Psychiatry Branch, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Iowa School of Medicine, Iowa City, Iowa. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Shaw, Child Psychiatry Branch, 10 Center Drive, Bldg. 10 3N202, Bethesda, MD 20892-1600; [email protected] (e-mail).

Dr. Giedd has received an honorarium from Johnson and Johnson. All other authors report no competing interests.

References

1. Kessler RC, Adler L, Barkley R, Biederman J, Conners CK, Demler O, Faraone SV, Greenhill LL, Howes MJ, Secnik K, Spencer T, Ustun TB, Walters EE, Zaslavsky AM: The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163:716–723Google Scholar

2. Castellanos FX, Lee PP, Sharp W, Jeffries NO, Greenstein DK, Clasen LS, Blumenthal JD, James RS, Ebens CL, Walter JM, Zijdenbos A, Evans AC, Giedd JN, Rapoport JL: Developmental trajectories of brain volume abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA 2002; 288:1740–1748Google Scholar

3. Berquin PC, Giedd JN, Jacobsen LK, Hamburger SD, Krain AL, Rapoport JL, Castellanos FX: Cerebellum in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a morphometric MRI study. Neurology 1998; 50:1087–1093Google Scholar

4. Bussing R, Grudnik J, Mason D, Wasiak M, Leonard C: ADHD and conduct disorder: an MRI study in a community sample. World J Biol Psychiatry 2002; 3:216–220Google Scholar

5. Castellanos FX, Giedd JN, Berquin PC, Walter JM, Sharp W, Tran T, Vaituzis AC, Blumenthal JD, Nelson J, Bastain TM, Zijdenbos A, Evans AC, Rapoport JL: Quantitative brain magnetic resonance imaging in girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001; 58:289–295Google Scholar

6. Hill DE, Yeo RA, Campbell RA, Hart B, Vigil J, Brooks W: Magnetic resonance imaging correlates of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children. Neuropsychology 2003; 17:496–506Google Scholar

7. Mostofsky SH, Reiss AL, Lockhart P, Denckla MB: Evaluation of cerebellar size in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Child Neurol 1998; 13:434–439Google Scholar

8. Ashtari M, Kumra S, Bhaskar SL, Clarke T, Thaden E, Cervellione KL, Rhinewine J, Kane JM, Adesman A, Milanaik R, Maytal J, Diamond A, Szeszko P, Ardekani BA: Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a preliminary diffusion tensor imaging study. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57:448–455Google Scholar

9. Harrington DL, Lee RR, Boyd LA, Rapcsak SZ, Knight RT: Does the representation of time depend on the cerebellum? effect of cerebellar stroke. Brain 2004; 127:561–574Google Scholar

10. Schmahmann JD: Disorders of the cerebellum: ataxia, dysmetria of thought, and the cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2004; 16:367–378Google Scholar

11. Ivry RB, Spencer RM, Zelaznik HN, Diedrichsen J: The cerebellum and event timing. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2002; 978:302–317Google Scholar

12. Rubia K, Smith A: The neural correlates of cognitive time management: a review. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) 2004; 64:329–340Google Scholar

13. Castellanos FX, Tannock R: Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: the search for endophenotypes. Nat Rev Neurosci 2002; 3:617–628Google Scholar

14. Nigg JT, Casey BJ: An integrative theory of attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder based on the cognitive and affective neurosciences. Dev Psychopathol 2005; 17:785–806Google Scholar

15. Pierson R, Corson PW, Sears LL, Alicata D, Magnotta V, Oleary D, Andreasen NC: Manual and semiautomated measurement of cerebellar subregions on MR images. Neuroimage 2002; 17:61–76Google Scholar

16. Allen G, Buxton RB, Wong EC, Courchesne E: Attentional activation of the cerebellum independent of motor involvement. Science 1997; 275:1940–1943Google Scholar

17. Shaw P, Lerch J, Greenstein D, Sharp W, Clasen L, Evans A, Giedd J, Castellanos FX, Rapoport J: Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and clinical outcome in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006; 63:540–549Google Scholar

18. Reich W: Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents (DICA). J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2000; 39:59–66Google Scholar

19. Werry JS, Sprague RL, Cohen MN: Conners’ Teacher Rating Scale for use in drug studies with children: an empirical study. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1975; 3:217–229Google Scholar

20. Shaffer D, Gould MS, Brasic J, Ambrosini P, Fisher P, Bird H, Aluwahlia S: A Children’s Global Assessment Scale (CGAS). Arch Gen Psychiatry 1983; 40:1228–1231Google Scholar

21. Sled JG, Zijdenbos AP, Evans AC: A nonparametric method for automatic correction of intensity nonuniformity in MRI data. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 1998; 17:87–97Google Scholar

22. Zijdenbos AP, Forghani R, Evans AC: Automatic “pipeline” analysis of 3-D MRI data for clinical trials: application to multiple sclerosis. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 2002; 21:1280–1291Google Scholar

23. Collins DL, Holmes CJ, Peters TM, Evans AC: Automatic 3-D model-based neuroanatomical segmentation. Hum Brain Mapp 1995; 3:190–208Google Scholar

24. Collins DL, Neelin P, Peters TM, Evans AC: Automatic 3D intersubject registration of MR volumetric data in standardized Talairach space. J Comput Assist Tomogr 1994; 18:192–205Google Scholar

25. Magnotta VA, Harris G, Andreasen NC, O’Leary DS, Yuh WT, Heckel D: Structural MR image processing using the BRAINS2 toolbox. Comput Med Imaging Graph 2002; 26:251–264Google Scholar

26. Larsell O, Jansen J: The Comparative Anatomy and Histology of the Cerebellum: The Human Cerebellum, Cerebellar Connections, and the Cerebellar Cortex. Minneapolis, University of Minnesota Press, 1972Google Scholar

27. Cohen J: Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hinsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum, 1988Google Scholar

28. Ivry RB, Keele SW, Diener HC: Dissociation of the lateral and medial cerebellum in movement timing and movement execution. Exp Brain Res 1988; 73:167–180Google Scholar

29. Golla H, Thier P, Haarmeier T: Disturbed overt but normal covert shifts of attention in adult cerebellar patients. Brain 2005; 128:1525–1535Google Scholar

30. Townsend J, Courchesne E, Covington J, Westerfield M, Harris NS, Lyden P, Lowry TP, Press GA: Spatial attention deficits in patients with acquired or developmental cerebellar abnormality. J Neurosci 1999; 19:5632–5643Google Scholar

31. Desmond JE, Gabrieli JD, Wagner AD, Ginier BL, Glover GH: Lobular patterns of cerebellar activation in verbal working-memory and finger-tapping tasks as revealed by functional MRI. J Neurosci 1997; 17:9675–9685Google Scholar

32. Riva D, Giorgi C: The cerebellum contributes to higher functions during development: evidence from a series of children surgically treated for posterior fossa tumours. Brain 2000; 123:1051–1061Google Scholar

33. Cabeza R, Nyberg L: Imaging cognition, II: an empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. J Cogn Neurosci 2000; 12:1–47Google Scholar

34. Desmond JE: Neuroimaging studies of the cerebellum: language, learning, and memory. Trends Cogn Sci 1998; 2:355–362Google Scholar

35. Van Harskamp NJ, Rudge P, Cipolotti L: Cognitive and social impairments in patients with superficial siderosis. Brain 2005; 128:1082–1092Google Scholar

36. Jueptner M, Rijntjes M, Weiller C, Faiss JH, Timmann D, Mueller SP, Diener HC: Localization of a cerebellar timing process using PET. Neurology 1995; 45:1540–1545Google Scholar

37. Ackermann H, Graber S, Hertrich I, Daum I: Cerebellar contributions to the perception of temporal cues within the speech and nonspeech domain. Brain Lang 1999; 67:228–241Google Scholar

38. Maquet P, Lejeune H, Pouthas V, Bonnet M, Casini L, Macar F, Timsit-Berthier M, Vidal F, Ferrara A, Degueldre C, Quaglia L, Delfiore G, Luxen A, Woods R, Mazziotta JC, Comar D: Brain activation induced by estimation of duration: a PET study. Neuroimage 1996; 3:119–126Google Scholar

39. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Fossella J, Flombaum JI, Posner MI: The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage 2005; 26:471–479Google Scholar

40. Rubia K, Noorloos J, Smith A, Gunning B, Sergeant J: Motor timing deficits in community and clinical boys with hyperactive behavior: the effect of methylphenidate on motor timing. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2003; 31:301–313Google Scholar

41. Smith A, Taylor E, Rogers JW, Newman S, Rubia K: Evidence for a pure time perception deficit in children with ADHD. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2002; 43:529–542Google Scholar

42. Sonuga-Barke EJ, Saxton T, Hall M: The role of interval underestimation in hyperactive children’s failure to suppress responses over time. Behav Brain Res 1998; 94:45–50Google Scholar

43. Rubia K, Taylor A, Taylor E, Sergeant JA: Synchronization, anticipation, and consistency in motor timing of children with dimensionally defined attention deficit hyperactivity behaviour. Percept Mot Skills 1999; 89:1237–1258Google Scholar

44. Barkley RA, Koplowitz S, Anderson T, McMurray MB: Sense of time in children with ADHD: effects of duration, distraction, and stimulant medication. J Int Neuropsychol 1997; 3:359–369Google Scholar

45. Schulz KP, Fan J, Tang CY, Newcorn JH, Buchsbaum MS, Cheung AM, Halperin JM: Response inhibition in adolescents diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during childhood: an event-related fMRI study. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:1650–1657Google Scholar

46. Silveri MC, Di Betta AM, Filippini V, Leggio MG, Molinari M: Verbal short-term store-rehearsal system and the cerebellum: evidence from a patient with a right cerebellar lesion. Brain 1998; 121:2175–2187Google Scholar

47. Ravizza SM, McCormick CA, Schlerf JE, Justus T, Ivry RB, Fiez JA: Cerebellar damage produces selective deficits in verbal working memory. Brain 2006; 129:306–320Google Scholar

48. Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Nigg JT, Faraone SV, Pennington BF: Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57:1336–1346Google Scholar

49. Xu D, Liu T, Ashe J, Bushara KO: Role of the olivo-cerebellar system in timing. J Neurosci 2006; 26:5990–5995Google Scholar

50. Makino Y, Yokosawa K, Takeda Y, Kumada T: Visual search and memory search engage extensive overlapping cerebral cortices: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 2004; 23:525–533Google Scholar

51. Valera EM, Faraone SV, Biederman J, Poldrack RA, Seidman LJ: Functional neuroanatomy of working memory in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57:439–447Google Scholar

52. Wallace GL, Schmitt JE, Lenroot R, Viding E, Ordaz S, Rosenthal MA, Molloy EA, Clasen L, Kendler KS, Neale MC, Giedd JN: A pediatric twin study of brain morphometry. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2006; 47:987–993Google Scholar

53. Mill J, Caspi A, Williams BS, Craig I, Taylor A, Polo-Tomas M, Berridge CW, Poulton R, Moffitt TE: Prediction of heterogeneity in intelligence and adult prognosis by genetic polymorphisms in the dopamine system among children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: evidence from 2 birth cohorts. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006; 63:462–469Google Scholar

54. Holmes J, Payton A, Barrett J, Harrington R, McGuffin P, Owen M, Ollier W, Worthington J, Gill M, Kirley A, Hawi Z, Fitzgerald M, Asherson P, Curran S, Mill J, Gould A, Taylor E, Kent L, Craddock N, Thapar A: Association of DRD4 in children with ADHD and comorbid conduct problems. Am J Med Genetics 2002; 114:150–153Google Scholar

55. Fleiss JL: Statistical Methods for Rates and Proportions. New York, John Wiley, 1991Google Scholar