Skip to main content

Association of RGS4 variants with schizotypy and cognitive endophenotypes at the population level

Abstract

Background

While association studies on schizophrenia show conflicting results regarding the importance of the regulator of the G-protein signaling 4 (RGS4) gene, recent work suggests that RGS4 may impact on the structural and functional integrity of the prefrontal cortex. We aimed to study associations of common RGS4 variants with prefrontal dependent cognitive performance and schizotypy endophenotypes at the population level.

Methods

Four RGS4 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNP1 [rs10917670], SNP4 [rs951436], SNP7 [rs951439], and SNP18 [rs2661319]) and their haplotypes were selected. Their associations with self-rated schizotypy (SPQ), vigilance, verbal, spatial working memory and antisaccade eye performance were tested with regressions in a representative population of 2,243 young male military conscripts.

Results

SNP4 was associated with negative schizotypy (higher SPQ negative factor for common T allele, p = 0.009; p = 0.031 for differences across genotypes) and a similar trend was seen also for common A allele of SNP18 (p = 0.039 for allele-load model; but p = 0.12 for genotype differences). Haplotype analyses showed a similar pattern with a dose-response for the most common haplotype (GGGG) on the negative schizotypy score with or without adjustment for age, IQ and their interaction (p = 0.011 and p = 0.024, respectively). There was no clear evidence for any association of the RGS4 variants with cognitive endophenotypes, except for an isolated effect of SNP18 on antisaccade error rate (p = 0.028 for allele-load model).

Conclusion

Common RGS4 variants were associated with negative schizotypal personality traits amongst a large cohort of young healthy individuals. In accordance with recent findings, this may suggest that RGS4 variants impact on the functional integrity of the prefrontal cortex, thus increasing susceptibility for psychotic spectrum disorders.

Background

Over the past few years conflicting evidence has been obtained concerning the association between the gene encoding the regulator of G-protein signaling 4 (RGS4) and schizophrenia. Initially evidence for linkage with schizophrenia was reported near RGS4 at 1q21-22 [1, 2] and several association studies also suggested modest associations for certain RGS4 gene variants [3–9]. However, a follow-up investigation of the original linkage study found no associations with RGS4 in this same sample [10] and many other studies found also negative results. Three recent meta-analyses either show no association between RGS4 and schizophrenia or suggest modest effects for SNP4 (rs951436) and for two common haplotypes [11–13]. Updated evaluation of the relevant association data shows no significant association for any of the 4 most commonly studied polymorphisms [14].

A plausible hypothesis to explain the inconsistencies in these studies is that RGS4 variants may modulate endophenotypes associated with schizophrenia rather than risk of disease itself [15]. Endophenotypes are measurable components along the pathway between the genetic infrastructure and the presentation of a disorder. Candidate endophenotypes of schizophrenia with which the disorder presumably shares a degree of overlapping genetic liability include structural and morphometric brain alterations, neurocognitive deficits and schizotypal personality traits or symptoms. These are expressed as quantitative traits with different intensities across a broad phenotypic spectrum, ranging from patients to their unaffected relatives and extending to the (disease-free) general population. Recent evidence that RGS4 may indeed impact on such endophenotypes is provided by. Prasad et al [16] who reported that left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) volumes were significantly different across first episode schizophrenia patients and controls on SNPs 4 (rs951436) and 18 (rs2661319) after correcting for multiple comparisons, indicating that these RGS4 polymorphisms may contribute to structural alterations in brain areas, previously associated with schizophrenia. Buckholtz et al [17] provided further evidence that SNP 4 (rs951436) impacts frontoparietal and frontotemporal response during working memory (n-Back test) and regionally specific reductions in gray and white matter structural volume in healthy individuals carrying the common risk allele. These studies indicate that structural and functional integrity of the prefrontal cortex might be associated with RGS4 polymorphic variation.

We have recently reported on the utility of adopting a population endophenotype approach to study the potential effect of candidate susceptibility genes for schizophrenia [18]. We hypothesize that endophenotypes for schizophrenia such as schizotypal personality traits [19], cognitive ability dependent on the integrity of prefrontal cortex such as sustained attention [20], working memory [21] and antisaccade eye-movements [22], may serve as potential targets of schizophrenia susceptibility genes. In this study, we set out to evaluate in a large cohort of apparently healthy young males whether 4 single nucleotide polymorphic variability in the RGS4 gene locus that has been previously associated with schizophrenia susceptibility, does modulate the expression of such schizophrenia related endophenotypes. We specifically hypothesized based on recent neuroimaging reports that RGS4 common "risk" alleles and haplotypes would be associated with a relative impairment of prefrontally mediated psychological and/or neuropsychological function.

Methods

Population

Details on the study population, assessments, and outcomes have been presented previously [18]. Briefly, the Athens Study of Psychosis Proneness and Incidence of Schizophrenia (ASPIS) examined 2243 randomly selected young male conscripts aged 18 to 24 years from the Greek Air Force in their first 2 weeks of admission to the National Basic Air Force Training Center. Conscripts underwent an extensive interview of computerized neurocognitive abilities and a self-rated psychometric evaluation. After obtaining written informed consent, DNA was extracted from mouthwash samples. This study was approved by the Bioethics and Medical Deontology Committee of the University Mental Health Research Institute.

Assessments

The assessment battery included, among other (see below), the Raven Progressive Matrices [23] and the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ) [18, 24, 25]. Each subject performed cognitive tasks and eye-movement tasks These included assessment of verbal and spatial versions of the n-back task to assess verbal and spatial working memory [26, 27] and anti-saccade eye movement task [28].

In this report we chose not to analyse other available population phenotypes that are not considered to be directly related to prefrontal function, such as general measure of state psychopathology, self rated personality scales, subclinical psychotic symptoms (Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences [29], and additional eye tracking measures.

We excluded from analyses individuals whose responses did not have face validity, based on a priori specified criteria. These were a negative central index of performance in 2-back tests; three or more unsuccessful trials (out of five) for verbal and spatial 2-back; and at least 50 invalid out of 90 antisaccade trials. We also excluded random responders in the TCI questionnaire [30], i.e., subjects who responded incorrectly in at least one out of four validity items.

DNA extraction, SNP selection and genotyping

Mouthwash samples for DNA extraction were chosen as described previously [31] to obtain a better procedure acceptance rate. For RGS4 we selected the four SNPs reported originally by Chowdari et al [3], namely SNP1 (rs10917670), SNP4 (rs951436), SNP7 (rs951439), and SNP18 (rs2661319). These SNPs have been assessed practically in all subsequent association studies on this gene [11].

All genotyping was performed blind to phenotype measures by K-Biosciences [32] using a competitive allele-specific PCR system (CASP). Genotyping was conducted on all markers and then a quality control procedure was applied using a simple statistical exclusion protocol for removal of poor quality DNA samples: the call rate as a percentage of SNP's genotyped were determined on a sample by sample basis. DNA samples that failed to call in 60% or more SNP's were excluded from the data set to improve reliability and accuracy of the data. Regarding inter and intra plate duplication. 5.8% of the genotyping data was repeated both within 384 well PCR plates and between 384 PCR plates. This repetition showed 3 non-concordant calls indicating an error rate of < 0.1%.

Outcomes – Statistical analysis

We quantified schizotypal traits as continuous measurements with the total score from the SPQ instrument. We assessed separately the four schizotypal latent dimensions of SPQ that emerged from applying confirmatory factor analysis to this dataset (cognitive/perceptual, negative, disorganization and paranoid factor) [25]. Moreover, we measured neurocognitive performance on four tasks: sustained attention, short-term spatial and verbal working memory and antisaccade eye movements. These measures were quantified using the corresponding sensitivity indexes: d'-CPT_IP, d'-S2B, d'-V2B and antisaccade error rate, respectively.

For every SNP we assessed compliance with the Hardy-Weinberg (HW) law using an exact test. We estimated the D' and r2 coefficients for SNP pairs using Haploview 3.2 [33]. Haplotypes (SNP1-SNP7-SNP4-SNP18) for each gene were reconstructed with PHASE 2.1.1 [34, 35]. Convergence of the Markov Chain Monte Carlo chains in PHASE was assessed by multiple (at least 5) runs. Haplotypes with frequency < 5% were grouped in a miscellaneous ("rare") category.

We checked for systematic differences in genotype and haplotype frequencies between those conscripts who were finally eligible and those who were excluded based on their random or incomplete questionnaire responses. Differences in the distributions were tested with chi-squared tests for individual SNPs and with a permutation test implemented in PHASE for haplotypes [34].

We assessed the impact of genetic factors on phenotype with regression models. Both single polymorphism analyses and haplotype-based analyses were performed. In single polymorphism evaluations, the distribution of each outcome per genotypic group was assessed with analysis of variance (ANOVA); this is a model-free approach that does not presume a specific model of genetic effect. We also examined with linear regressions whether the number of minor allele copies was associated with each outcome (allele-load or allele-based additive models). In haplotype-based assessments, we also relied on a likelihood ratio test (LR p value) to assess whether a regression model taking into account the genetic factors provided better fit (explained data better) than a constant only model. In the regression models, the most common haplotype was used as the reference category. This choice was arbitrary but is mathematically equivalent to choosing any haplotype as the reference category. In addition we contrasted the most common haplotype against all other haplotypes combined. Sensitivity analyses weighted each haplotype according to its probability as calculated by PHASE.

The main analyses did not adjust for non-genetic factors. However, in a secondary analysis, we adjusted for age and IQ and their interaction. In the latter case, we based inferences on the presence of genetic effects by assessing with a likelihood ratio test whether a model that takes age, IQ and gene haplotypes into account provides better fit than a model based on age and IQ only.

Association analyses were performed in Intercooled Stata 8.2 [36]. The reported p-values are 2-tailed and uncorrected for multiple comparisons.

For associations that passed the threshold of statistical significance at the p = 0.05 level, we also estimated the respective Bayes factors. We followed the method of Ioannidis [37] where the prior is specified as a spike null and smear alternative. The spike is placed at the null and the smear is normally distributed around the null. The Bayes factor can be readily estimated from the observed z-value (standardized effect) of the associaiton, the variance of the observed effect, and the assumed average value of the genetic effects in a given direction under the alternative. We applied these calculations illustratively for significant association in the allele-based model. The inverse of the Bayes factor shows how many times the odds that an association is true increases compared to the prior odds before running the study.

Results

Descriptive data

In total, 2243 randomly selected young male conscripts (mean age, 20.7 +- 1.90) entered the study. As previously described in more detail [18] the proportion of conscripts who gave eligible responses and measurements varied between 60% for SPQ and 90% for cognitive measurements.

Genotyping was successful for 81.4% on rs10917670, 88.2% on rs951436, 89.8% on rs951439, and 78.7% on rs2661319. Genotype availability was not significantly related to any of the instrument scores (p-values ranged between 0.64 and 1.00 for all analyses).

Genotype frequencies were as follows. For rs10917670: AA = 325, AG = 842, GG = 522; for rs951436 GG = 374, GT = 927, TT = 531; for rs951439: AA = 353, AG = 949, GG = 563; and for rs2661319: GG = 379, AG = 813, and AA = 441. No significant deviations from the HW law were seen for any of the 4 tested polymorphisms (p > 0.19 for all).

Strong linkage disequilibrium was observed between rs10917670 and rs951439 (r2 = 0.98). r2 values ranged between 0.51 and 0.81 for all other SNP pairs. In accordance with previous reported studies on individuals of Caucasian origin, the most common haplotypes were GGGG (44.0%), ATAA (40.8%), and GTGA (9.9%); all other haplotypes occurred in 3% or less of the population.

Associations with individual SNPs – model-free approach

In model-free analyses (analyses that compare outcome distributions across the three genotypes for each SNP) a single association between SNP4 (rs951436) and the negative dimension of schizotypal personality reached nominal statistical significance (p = 0.031).

Associations with individual SNPs – allele-load models

As shown in Table 1, there were three nominally statistically significant signals when analyses used an additive allele-load model. Specifically the T allele of SNP4 (rs951436) and the A allele of SNP18 were associated with an increase in negative schizotypy (p = 0.009 and p = 0.039, respectively). The latter was also associated with an increase in antisaccade error rate (p = 0.028). Effect sizes were modest for these nominally significant findings. The three nominally statistically significant associations (p = 0.009, 0.039 and 0.028) had Bayes factors of 0.15, 0.43, and 0.30, respectively, when the genetic effect under the alternative was assumed to have an average value in the negative direction of -0.01, -0.01, and -0.02 respectively (the magnitude of the effects estimates that were observed).

Table 1 Effect sizes (beta) per minor allele copy for RGS4 gene SNPs

Haplotype-based analyses

Overall, haplotype analyses yielded consistent results with SNP-based analyses (Table 2 and Table 3). In main analyses, likelihood ratio tests suggested a non-significant relationship between RGS4 haplotypes and the negative dimension of SPQ (p = 0.06; Table 2). The reference haplotype GGGG (that does not include the T allele of SNP4 and the A allele of SNP18) was associated with lower values in the negative dimension of SPQ, compared to the common ATAA and GTGA haplotypes (p = 0.03; Table 2), or compared to all other haplotypes combined (p = 0.01; Table 3), albeit with modest effects. In addition, non-significant trends for a relationship of haplotypes with spatial working memory were also observed (likelihood ratio testing, p = 0.07; Table 2). Adjusted analyses were qualitatively similar for the aforementioned outcomes.

Table 2 Haplotypes based on best pairs (unweighted) UNADJUSTED
Table 3 Most Common haplotype vs all other haplotypes based on best pairs (unweighted)

Weighting for the probability of each haplotype (Table 4 and Table 5) suggested a trend for an association of haplotypes with spatial working memory (likelihood ratio tests, p = 0.048 without and p = 0.077 with adjustments). Specifically, each copy of the GTGA haplotype was associated with slightly superior spatial working memory performance (0.16 points) compared to individuals with the most common haplotype GGGG. Haplotype analyses limited to haplotypes where all SNPs were available versus including also those where some SNPs were missing, yielded similar results.

Table 4 Haplotypes weighting for the probability of each haplotype UNADJUSTED
Table 5 Haplotypes weighting for the probability of each haplotype ADJUSTED for age, IQ and interaction

Discussion

Our analysis has found that RGS4 variants exhibit some tentative signals for association with endophenotypes that might be relevant to the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Common 'risk' alleles of SNP4 and SNP18, the SNPs that have been previously related also to reduced prefrontal cortex volume and function, were associated in the current study with an increase in negative schizotypal personality traits amongst a large population of apparently healthy young male conscripts. Haplotype analysis generally supported this association. An isolated model-specific effect of risk allele A (SNP18) on antisaccade error rate was noted. The SNPs 1 and 7 that were not associated with reduced brain volumes in a previous study [16] had no effect on cognitive or schizotypy endophenotypes.

Negative schizotypy personality traits in the general population similarly to their illness counterpart (negative symptoms in schizophrenia), are considered to be associated mostly with subtle deficits of prefrontal brain function. This is indicated by the modest but persistent correlation of this schizotypy factor to executive dysfunction and working memory deficits [38–40]. If RGS4 variants truly contribute to structural and functional alterations of the prefrontal cortex [16, 17], the observed association of RGS4 risk variants with negative schizotypy may reflect such an impact on prefrontal function. Furthermore, the dose effect of risk allele A of SNP18 on negative schizotypy and simultaneously on antisaccade error rate (arguably a good index of Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex integrity) provides some further support to the notion that RGS4 risk variants are associated with subtle deficits at the information processing level surveyed by the prefrontal cortex. A similar pleiotropic effect of SNP4 on negative psychotic symptoms and tasks dependant on the integrity of prefrontal function (verbal fluency) was also noted in a recent association study [9] although these associations did not survive multiple comparison testing. On the other hand, we observed no clear effect of RGS4 polymorphisms on 2- Back working memory indices of performance as in a recent study [17], but nevertheless haplotype analyses offered a soft signal of association with spatial working memory.

RGS4 variation was associated with negative rather than positive schizotypy. These two factors may have relatively independent neurocognitive [40] neurochemical [41] and genetic [42, 43] underpinnings. Negative symptoms in schizophrenia lay in an etiological continuum with their personality counterpart [44] and may have greater familiar, neurodevelopmental and possibly genetic basis than positive ones. Interestingly the first emerging association studies which include clinical and neurocognitive outcomes, also suggest associations of SNP4 or SNP18 with PANSS global psychopathology scores [9, 45], negative PANSS symptoms and simultaneous association with tasks dependent of prefrontal brain function (verbal fluency) and impaired neurodevelopment (premorbid verbal IQ) [9].

If RGS4 variation has only modest effects on negative schizotypy endophenotypes, as observed in our study, this may explain the inconsistency of the previous association studies and the lack of a demonstrable strong association with schizophrenia per se, when these studies have been combined in meta-analyses [14]. Depending on the population mix and the importance that these endophenotypes may play in the disease process, some case-control studies of schizophrenia may show subtle effects with RGS4 variants, while others may show none. Genetic effects on schizophrenia risk may be very small on average and may require thousands of several tens of thousands of subjects to document or refute convincingly [46]. Conversely, quantitative traits such as these endophenotypes would be possible to study efficiently with relatively smaller sample sizes. Still, the importance of replicating our results in other cohorts cannot be over-emphasize [47, 48]. Conclusive results are likely to require considerable sample sizes even for these endophenotypes, and in the presence of population or other heterogeneity, consistent replication may be difficult [49].

Along these lines, we acknowledge that, several, if not all, of the identified signals in our study might be false positives [50, 51]. With 4 SNPs and 10 outcome variables, there are 40 sets of analyses performed, even without consideration of haplotypes. However, 2 SNPs were in almost perfect linkage disequilibrium and many of the outcomes also show high correlation with each other. Therefore, the multiplicity of comparisons is far less than implied at first sight. Moreover, our approach was to target variants of a gene that already had some indirect or direct support for involvement in the pathogenesis of schizophrenia and therefore the pre-study probability of significant associations was not negligible as in a hypothesis-free, discovery-oriented approach. Regardless, the interpretation of the modestly significant associations should be conservative. In addition, the Bayes factors obtained for the nominally significant associations are not very strong, but they should not be disregarded, given the prior evidence on potential implication of this genes and variants in related phenotypes.

We should also acknowledge that observed associations may exist due to hitherto unidentified risk variants at RGS4 that are in strong linkage disequilibrium with these four SNPs. All four SNPs in the associated haplotype are non-coding SNPs, but SNPs 1, 4 and 7 are located in a 5' region of the gene that may play a role in transcription regulation.

Another limitation of our study was the considerable rate of non-responders for some items, especially the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire and the loss of some information due to failure of genotyping in some mouthwash samples. These missing data may have eroded some of the power of the study to detect statistically significant genetic effects. However, there is no reason why genotypes would be missed preferentially in participants with specific phenotypes and indeed we found no hint of such association. Similarly, we found no evidence that phenotype data were missing based on phenotype. Therefore, missing data is unlikely to have generated false-positives.

Conclusion

Allowing for these caveats, our study suggests that SNP4 and SNP18 may affect some yet unidentified prefrontal-mediated neuronal mechanism that predisposes apparently healthy individuals to report higher scores on items related to social isolation, and interpersonal difficulties. These genetic effects seem to be modest, but they are definitely worth of further evaluation, since they would pertain to the wide general population of otherwise healthy individuals.

References

  1. Gurling HM, Kalsi G, Brynjolfson J, Sigmundsson T, Sherrington R, Mankoo BS, Read T, Murphy P, Blaveri E, McQuillin A, Petursson H, Curtis D: Genomewide genetic linkage analysis confirms the presence of susceptibility loci for schizophrenia, on chromosomes 1q32.2, 5q33.2, and 8p21-22 and provides support for linkage to schizophrenia, on chromosomes 11q23.3-24 and 20q12.1-11.23. Am J Hum Genet. 2001, 68: 661-673. 10.1086/318788.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Brzustowicz LM, Hayter JE, Hodgkinson KA, Chow EW, Bassett AS: Fine mapping of the schizophrenia susceptibility locus on chromosome 1q22. Hum Hered. 2002, 54: 199-209. 10.1159/000070665.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Chowdari KV, Mirnics K, Semwal P, Wood J, Lawrence E, Bhatia T, Deshpande SN, B KT, Ferrell RE, Middleton FA, Devlin B, Levitt P, Lewis DA, Nimgaonkar VL: Association and linkage analyses of RGS4 polymorphisms in schizophrenia. Hum Mol Genet. 2002, 11: 1373-1380. 10.1093/hmg/11.12.1373.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Williams NM, Preece A, Spurlock G, Norton N, Williams HJ, McCreadie RG, Buckland P, Sharkey V, Chowdari KV, Zammit S, Nimgaonkar V, Kirov G, Owen MJ, O'Donovan MC: Support for RGS4 as a susceptibility gene for schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2004, 55: 192-195. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.11.002.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Morris DW, Rodgers A, McGhee KA, Schwaiger S, Scully P, Quinn J, Meagher D, Waddington JL, Gill M, Corvin AP: Confirming RGS4 as a susceptibility gene for schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2004, 125: 50-53. 10.1002/ajmg.b.20109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Chen X, Dunham C, Kendler S, Wang X, O'Neill FA, Walsh D, Kendler KS: Regulator of G-protein signaling 4 (RGS4) gene is associated with schizophrenia in Irish high density families. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2004, 129: 23-26. 10.1002/ajmg.b.30078.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Zhang F, St Clair D, Liu X, Sun X, Sham PC, Crombie C, Ma X, Wang Q, Meng H, Deng W, Yates P, Hu X, Walker N, Murray RM, Collier DA, Li T: Association analysis of the RGS4 gene in Han Chinese and Scottish populations with schizophrenia. Genes Brain Behav. 2005, 4: 444-448. 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00167.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Fallin MD, Lasseter VK, Avramopoulos D, Nicodemus KK, Wolyniec PS, McGrath JA, Steel G, Nestadt G, Liang KY, Huganir RL, Valle D, Pulver AE: Bipolar I disorder and schizophrenia: a 440-single-nucleotide polymorphism screen of 64 candidate genes among Ashkenazi Jewish case-parent trios. Am J Hum Genet. 2005, 77: 918-936. 10.1086/497703.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. So HC, Chen RY, Chen EY, Cheung EF, Li T, Sham PC: An association study of RGS4 polymorphisms with clinical phenotypes of schizophrenia in a Chinese population. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2008, 147B (1): 77-85. 10.1002/ajmg.b.30577.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Brzustowicz LM, Simone J, Mohseni P, Hayter JE, Hodgkinson KA, Chow EW, Bassett AS: Linkage disequilibrium mapping of schizophrenia susceptibility to the CAPON region of chromosome 1q22. Am J Hum Genet. 2004, 74: 1057-1063. 10.1086/420774.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Talkowski ME, Seltman H, Bassett AS, Brzustowicz LM, Chen X, Chowdari KV, Collier DA, Cordeiro Q, Corvin AP, Deshpande SN, Egan MF, Gill M, Kendler KS, Kirov G, Heston LL, Levitt P, Lewis DA, Li T, Mirnics K, Morris DW, Norton N, O'Donovan MC, Owen MJ, Richard C, Semwal P, Sobell JL, St Clair D, Straub RE, Thelma BK, Vallada H, Weinberger DR, Williams NM, Wood J, Zhang F, Devlin B, Nimgaonkar VL: Evaluation of a susceptibility gene for schizophrenia: genotype based meta-analysis of RGS4 polymorphisms from thirteen independent samples. Biol Psychiatry. 2006, 60: 152-162. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.02.015.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Li D, He L: Association study of the G-protein signaling 4 (RGS4) and proline dehydrogenase (PRODH) genes with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Eur J Hum Genet. 2006, 14: 1130-1135. 10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201680.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Guo S, Tang W, Shi Y, Huang K, Xi Z, Xu Y, Feng G, He L: RGS4 polymorphisms and risk of schizophrenia: an association study in Han Chinese plus meta-analysis. Neurosci Lett. 2006, 406: 122-127. 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.07.028.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Allen NC, Bagade S, McQueen MB, Ioannidis JP, Kavvoura FK, Khoury MJ, Tanzi RE, Bertram L: Systematic meta-analyses and field synopsis of genetic association studies in schizophrenia: the SzGene database. Nat Genet. 2008, 40: 827-834. 10.1038/ng.171.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Gottesman II, Gould TD: The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry. 2003, 160: 636-645. 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.636.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Prasad KM, Chowdari KV, Nimgaonkar VL, Talkowski ME, Lewis DA, Keshavan MS: Genetic polymorphisms of the RGS4 and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex morphometry among first episode schizophrenia patients. Mol Psychiatry. 2005, 10: 213-219. 10.1038/sj.mp.4001562.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Buckholtz JW, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Honea RA, Straub RE, Pezawas L, Egan MF, Vakkalanka R, Kolachana B, Verchinski BA, Sust S, Mattay VS, Weinberger DR, Callicott JH: Allelic variation in RGS4 impacts functional and structural connectivity in the human brain. J Neurosci. 2007, 27: 1584-1593. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5112-06.2007.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Stefanis NC, Trikalinos TA, Avramopoulos D, Smyrnis N, Evdokimidis I, Ntzani EE, Ioannidis JP, Stefanis CN: Impact of Schizophrenia Candidate Genes on Schizotypy and Cognitive Endophenotypes at the Population Level. Biol Psychiatry. 2007, 62: 784-792. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.015.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Stefanis NC, Vitoratou S, Ntzoufras I, Smyrnis N, Evdokimidis I, CN S: Psychometric properties of the Greek version of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ) in young male obligatory conscripts: A two years test-retest study. Pers Individ Dif. 2007, 41: 1275-1286. 10.1016/j.paid.2006.07.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Cornblatt BA, Risch NJ, Faris G, Friedman D, Erlenmeyer-Kimling L: The Continuous Performance Test, identical pairs version (CPT-IP): I. New findings about sustained attention in normal families. Psychiatry Res. 1988, 26: 223-238. 10.1016/0165-1781(88)90076-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Cannon TD, Huttunen MO, Lonnqvist J, Tuulio-Henriksson A, Pirkola T, Glahn D, Finkelstein J, Hietanen M, Kaprio J, Koskenvuo M: The inheritance of neuropsychological dysfunction in twins discordant for schizophrenia. Am J Hum Genet. 2000, 67: 369-382. 10.1086/303006.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Ettinger U, Picchioni M, Hall MH, Schulze K, Toulopoulou T, Landau S, Crawford TJ, Murray RM: Antisaccade performance in monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia: the Maudsley twin study. Am J Psychiatry. 2006, 163: 543-545. 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.3.543.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Raven J: Revised Manual for Raven's Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. 1982, Windsor: NFER-Nelson

    Google Scholar 

  24. Raine A: The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophr Bull. 1991, 17: 555-564.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Stefanis NC, Smyrnis N, Avramopoulos D, Evdokimidis I, Ntzoufras I, Stefanis CN: Factorial composition of self-rated schizotypal traits among young males undergoing military training. Schizophr Bull. 2004, 30: 335-350.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Gevins A, Smith ME, Le J, Leong H, Bennett J, Martin N, McEvoy L, Du R, Whitfield S: High resolution evoked potential imaging of the cortical dynamics of human working memory. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1996, 98: 327-348. 10.1016/0013-4694(96)00288-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Smyrnis N, Avramopoulos D, Evdokimidis I, Stefanis CN, Tsekou H, Stefanis NC: Effect of schizotypy on cognitive performance and its tuning by COMT val158 met genotype variations in a large population of young men. Biol Psychiatry. 2007, 61: 845-853. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.019.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Smyrnis N, Evdokimidis I, Stefanis NC, Avramopoulos D, Constantinidis TS, Stavropoulos A, Stefanis CN: Antisaccade performance of 1,273 men: effects of schizotypy, anxiety, and depression. J Abnorm Psychol. 2003, 112: 403-414. 10.1037/0021-843X.112.3.403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Stefanis NC, Hanssen M, Smirnis NK, Avramopoulos DA, Evdokimidis IK, Stefanis CN, Verdoux H, Van Os J: Evidence that three dimensions of psychosis have a distribution in the general population. Psychol Med. 2002, 32: 347-358. 10.1017/S0033291701005141.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR: A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993, 50: 975-990.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Avramopoulos D, Stefanis NC, Hantoumi I, Smyrnis N, Evdokimidis I, Stefanis CN: Higher scores of self reported schizotypy in healthy young males carrying the COMT high activity allele. Mol Psychiatry. 2002, 7: 706-711. 10.1038/sj.mp.4001070.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. KBioscience. http://www.kbioscience.co.uk

  33. Barrett JC, Fry B, Maller J, Daly MJ: Haploview: analysis and visualization of LD and haplotype maps. Bioinformatics. 2005, 21: 263-265. 10.1093/bioinformatics/bth457.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Stephens M, Smith NJ, Donnelly P: A new statistical method for haplotype reconstruction from population data. Am J Hum Genet. 2001, 68: 978-989. 10.1086/319501.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Stephens M, Scheet P: Accounting for decay of linkage disequilibrium in haplotype inference and missing-data imputation. Am J Hum Genet. 2005, 76: 449-462. 10.1086/428594.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Stata Corp, College Station, TX, USA. http://www.stata.com

  37. Ioannidis JP: Effect of formal statistical significance on the credibility of observational associations. Am J Epidemiol. 2008, 168 (4): 374-390. 10.1093/aje/kwn156.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Park S, McTigue K: Working memory and the syndromes of schizotypal personality. Schizophr Res. 1997, 26: 213-220. 10.1016/S0920-9964(97)00051-0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Suhr JA, Spitznagel MB: Factor versus cluster models of schizotypal traits. II: relation to neuropsychological impairment. Schizophr Res. 2001, 52: 241-250. 10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00185-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Dinn WM, Harris CL, Aycicegi A, Greene P, Andover MS: Positive and negative schizotypy in a student sample: neurocognitive and clinical correlates. Schizophr Res. 2002, 56: 171-185. 10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00230-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Siever LJ, ed.: Brain structure/function and the dopamine system in schizotypal personality disorder. 1995, New York: Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  42. Linney YM, Murray RM, Peters ER, MacDonald AM, Rijsdijk F, Sham PC: A quantitative genetic analysis of schizotypal personality traits. Psychol Med. 2003, 33: 803-816. 10.1017/S0033291703007906.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Lin CC, Su CH, Kuo PH, Hsiao CK, Soong WT, Chen WJ: Genetic and environmental influences on schizotypy among adolescents in Taiwan: a multivariate twin/sibling analysis. Behav Genet. 2007, 37: 334-344. 10.1007/s10519-006-9104-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Fanous A, Gardner C, Walsh D, Kendler KS: Relationship between positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia and schizotypal symptoms in nonpsychotic relatives. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001, 58: 669-673. 10.1001/archpsyc.58.7.669.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Campbell DB, Ebert PJ, Skelly T, Stroup TS, Lieberman J, Levitt P, Sullivan PF: Ethnic Stratification of the Association of RGS4 Variants with Antipsychotic Treatment Response in Schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2008, 63 (1): 32-41. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.018.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Ioannidis JP, Trikalinos TA, Khoury MJ: Implications of small effect sizes of individual genetic variants on the design and interpretation of genetic association studies of complex diseases. Am J Epidemiol. 2006, 164: 609-614. 10.1093/aje/kwj259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Ioannidis JP, Ntzani EE, Trikalinos TA, Contopoulos-Ioannidis DG: Replication validity of genetic association studies. Nat Genet. 2001, 29: 306-309. 10.1038/ng749.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Chanock SJ, Manolio T, Boehnke M, Boerwinkle E, Hunter DJ, Thomas G, Hirschhorn JN, Abecasis G, Altshuler D, Bailey-Wilson JE, Brooks LD, Cardon LR, Daly M, Donnelly P, Fraumeni JF, Freimer NB, Gerhard DS, Gunter C, Guttmacher AE, Guyer MS, Harris EL, Hoh J, Hoover R, Kong CA, Merikangas KR, Morton CC, Palmer LJ, Phimister EG, Rice JP, Roberts J, Rotimi C, Tucker MA, Vogan KJ, Wacholder S, Wijsman EM, Winn DM, Collins FS: Replicating genotype-phenotype associations. Nature. 2007, 447: 655-660. 10.1038/447655a.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Moonesinghe R, Khoury MJ, Liu T, Ioannidis JP: Required sample size and nonreplicability thresholds for heterogeneous genetic associations. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008, 105: 617-622. 10.1073/pnas.0705554105.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Ioannidis JP: Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Med. 2005, 2: e124-10.1371/journal.pmed.0020124.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Wacholder S, Chanock S, Garcia-Closas M, El Ghormli L, Rothman N: Assessing the probability that a positive report is false: an approach for molecular epidemiology studies. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2004, 96: 434-442.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Grant EKBAN 97 to C.N.S from the General Secretariat of Research and Technology of the Greek Ministry of Development. Intrasoft provided the technical support for this project

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nicholas C Stefanis.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

NS participated in the study protocol, overall design and execution of the study, first drafted this manuscript. TT organized and performed the bulk of statistical analyses as well as writing up part of this manuscript. DA was involved in data collection, and organizing the genetic arm of the study as well as reviewing this manuscript. NS and IE participated in the study protocol, data collection and dataset organization prior to statistical analyses, and result interpretation of this manuscript. ET contributed to snp, gene identification, background literature organization and preparation of this manuscript. AH participated in part of the statistical analysis and preparation of this manuscript. JI overviewed statistical analysis contributing also to writing part and reviewing this manuscript. CS is responsible for the overall conceptualization and supervision of the ASPIS study. He was involved in the interpretation of the results of this manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Stefanis, N.C., Trikalinos, T.A., Avramopoulos, D. et al. Association of RGS4 variants with schizotypy and cognitive endophenotypes at the population level. Behav Brain Funct 4, 46 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-4-46

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-4-46

Keywords