Main

Numerous studies report that breastfeeding is associated with higher scores on tests of neurodevelopment and cognition in later life (1,2), suggesting that breast milk may impact early brain development, with potentially important biologic, medical, and social implications. These findings are often questioned because other factors associated with mothers' choice to breastfeed (e.g. higher socioeconomic status and level of education, different child-rearing attitudes) might promote cognitive development. In a study using data from a national database, adjustment for maternal intelligence quotient (IQ) eliminated the breastfeeding effect (3). All these studies, however, are potentially flawed by lack of experimental design.

More recently, a large cluster randomized trial of breastfeeding promotion using an experimental design demonstrated a large effect of breastfeeding on cognition, adding credence to the evidence for beneficial effects seen in past observational studies (4). Anderson et al. (5) showed in a meta-analysis that, after appropriate adjustments, breastfeeding was associated with an advantage of around three points on tests of cognition in children born at term and around five points in those born preterm, both large effects in population terms. The implication is that, over and above social factors, one or more constituents of breast milk benefit neurodevelopment, particularly so in those born preterm, at a more sensitive stage of brain development.

Our current study was prompted by two factors, the now likely causal relationship between breastfeeding and cognition and the previous observation that cognitive scores in preterms are related to head circumference and brain size measured by quantitative volumetric analysis of MRI scans (6). With this background, we examined the relationships between breast milk feeding and cognition and brain volume determined from MRI scans. Our specific purpose was to provide evidence for the hypothesis that cognitive benefits of breastfeeding are mediated through an effect on brain growth, ultimately influencing mature brain volume.

To explore this hypothesis, we studied members of a cohort of adolescents who had participated in a large randomized trial examining the health and developmental effects of early infant nutrition, conducted between 1982 and 1985 (7). The subjects were born preterm, at a sensitive period for nutritional effects on brain development and cognition (8). In exploratory analyses, those receiving breast milk, after allowing for confounding factors, had an 8.3 point IQ advantage at 7–8 y (9). A subset of this cohort has been followed up to 13–19 y of age for MRI scanning and cognitive testing. Data collected on these subjects while in the neonatal unit provided precise information on the volumes of breast milk consumed (by nasogastric tube), giving a rare opportunity to explore the potential dose-response effect of breast milk feeding on brain volumes and cognition at adolescence. It is important to note that all neonatal data were recorded for this study at the time and were not obtained retrospectively from charts. Although some IQ data for the larger group have been reported previously (10,11), neither IQ data for this particular group nor information regarding breast milk in the diet have been published.

METHODS

Subjects.

The original randomized trial of early nutrition consisted of two studies, each with two parallel trials (8). In the trial of interest here, mothers elected to breastfeed but varied in their success in expressing milk and, therefore, in the need for supplementary feeding. According to randomization before breastfeeding commenced, these infants were given one of three supplements: a nutrient-enriched preterm formula (PTF), a standard term formula (TF; used in the 1980s for feeding preterm infants), or a banked breast milk (BBM; provided by unrelated donors and unfortified). The volumes of all enteral intakes were recorded daily (parenteral nutrition was not included) and the expressed maternal breast milk component converted to a percentage (%EBM) of the infant's total intake. The median number of days in the study was 39.5 (quartiles: 21.5; 39.5; 61.3).

Our intention was not to conduct another follow-up because the neuroimaging component constrained the number of adolescents who could be studied due to factors such as difficulty of travel to the hospital for MRI imaging. To make interpretation of the diet-brain relationships as straightforward as possible, we chose to include those who had been born at 30wk GA or less and who had been considered neurologically normal, by history and examination, when seen at 7.5–8 y, making them an atypical group. All attended mainstream schools. Cognitive and imaging data were available for 50 (26 M; 24 F) adolescents who had been assigned to the EBM + supplement limb of the original study, allocated to diet as follows: PTF = 28, TF = 13, and BBM = 9. Percentage EBM in the diet ranged from 0 (2 infants) to 100 (3 infants) with a mean of 60.1. Members of this group had been assessed cognitively in previous follow-up studies but no MRI scans were collected at time of birth (in the early 1980s) or subsequently. Mean age at cognitive testing/MRI acquisition was 15 y 9 mo (range: 13 y 5 mo–19 y 9 mo; SD = 13.6 mo).

Cognitive assessment.

The age-appropriate Wechsler IQ test was given: 44 completed the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition (WISC-III) and 6 the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition (WAIS-III). Mazes, an optional subtest, were omitted from the WISC-III. Verbal IQ (VIQ), performance IQ (PIQ), and full-scale IQ (FSIQ) scores were calculated. IQ scores have a population mean of 100 and a SD of 15.

MRI scan acquisition.

MRI studies were performed using a 1.5T Siemens Vision system. Investigations included magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (MPRAGE 3-D) (12) volume acquisition with repetition time of 10 ms; echo time, 4 ms; inversion time, 200 ms; flip angle, 12 degrees; matrix size, 256 × 256; field of view, 250 mm; partition thickness, 1.25 mm; 128 sagittal partitions in the third dimension, and acquisition time, 8.3 min.

MRI volumetric analysis.

We used an established technique developed by Fischl et al. (13) to obtain volumes for total brain (excluding cerebrospinal fluid) and for white and cortical gray matter in the left and right hemispheres separately; central gray matter and the cerebellum were not included. This method involves whole brain segmentation and the assignment of a neuroanatomical label to every point in the cortex; labeling of the structures, using T1-weighted images, is based on probabilistic information estimated from a training set manually labeled by neuroanatomists. It has been validated by correlation with histology, manual measures, and with known properties of the cortex (14,15). We correlated total brain volumes obtained on two separate occasions and, as expected from such an automated method, the correlation between them was 1.0.

Procedure.

Cognitive data were collected during the hospital visit for MRI acquisition. Test administration was carried out by one individual blind to %EBM. Written consent was obtained from all participants and parents/guardians, and approval was obtained from the relevant ethics committees (The Great Ormond Street NHS Trust/Institute of Child Health, Norwich District, South Sheffield Research, East Suffolk Local Research, and Cambridge Local Research Ethics Committees).

Statistics.

T-Tests were used to compare male and female groups. The relationships among %EBM, IQ scores, and neural volumes were calculated using partial correlation coefficients. In analyses involving %EBM and IQ scores, we covaried level of maternal education and social class. We did not correct for age at test because IQ scores are already age corrected or for GA because there was no significant relationship between GA and %EBM in this group (p = 0.2). In all analyses involving neural measurements from MRI scans, we added age at test/scan and GA as additional covariates because brain development takes place over the age range during which our adolescents were scanned (16) and because “baseline” brain structure at birth is related to GA (17). In fact, the relationships between GA and neural volumes were not significant in this group (the range in GA values was only 4 wks), but we included it in case of residual confounding. We then selected three quantitative variables reflecting aspects of neonatal condition: days to achieve enteral feeds of 150 mL/kg/d (only available for 41 infants), days of ventilation, and number of infections based on positive blood culture. We conducted principal analyses as described above for all children first, reporting these results. We then carried out secondary analyses with the additional covariates but only report these if the pattern of significant results was altered. We also conducted regression analyses in which all covariates were added as predictors.

RESULTS

Group characteristics.

Table 1 presents perinatal and milk intake data for the total group and genders separately; there were no significant differences between boys and girls. In addition, we looked at socioeconomic status (coded into 6 categories based on the UK Registrar General's Classification of Occupations) and level of maternal education (coded into 5 categories based on academic achievement). The distribution of education level did not differ between girls and boys; significantly more girls were classified as higher social class (χ2: p = 0.03). All mothers in the study had chosen to breastfeed and there was no relationship between either social class or maternal education and %EBM within the group. Nevertheless, to reduce any residual impact, both maternal education and social class were covaried in all subsequent analyses. Table 2 presents information specifically about markers of illness in the neonatal period that might have had an impact on feeding and subsequent outcomes. No child in this study received corticosteroids.

Table 1 Means (SD) for perinatal and social factors for the whole group and for males and females separately
Table 2 Indications of severity of illness during the neonatal course

The children had been classified as neurologically normal on the basis of history and examination but not MRI scan, at 7–8 y. Thirty-three of the 50 adolescent scans were considered normal on visual inspection. Of the remaining 17, 10 had a small corpus callosum, two had reduced bilateral hippocampi, two showed a mild periventricular leukomalacia pattern, and one each had prominent ventricles, left peritrigonal focal atrophy, and a possible MR signal abnormality in deep white matter. We could not discern any relationships between these findings and outcome measures.

As mentioned above, the group was chosen in such a way that it was not representative of the whole cohort. However, we compared the study group with the total group in the cohort who met the same selection criteria (GA ≤30 wk, normal neurologic status at 7–8 y and participated in trial 2; n = 84) to see if they were comparable: GA—total mean = 28.8 wks, study mean = 28.8 wks; birth weight—total mean = 1299 g, study mean = 1230 g; Apgar at 5 min—total mean = 7.7, study mean = 7.7; days of ventilation—total median = 3.0, study median = 2.0. The proportions of small for gestational age infants were 13% in the total group and 16% in the study group.

%EBM and IQ.

Table 3 shows that all mean IQ scores were close to the putative population mean of 100. IQ scores for boys and girls did not differ significantly. Correlation coefficients between %EBM and IQ measures are shown in Table 4; p levels ≤0.05 were considered significant. The whole group showed a significant relationship between %EBM and VIQ (FSIQ at trend level), largely attributable to the boys, who showed relationships between %EBM and all three IQ scores, whereas the girls showed none. Repeating these analyses with additional covariates did not change this pattern of results, except that the relationship between PIQ and %EBM in the boys was at trend level only (p = 0.08). We then carried out regression analyses for the three IQ scores with the following predictors: maternal education, social class, %EBM, number of infections, number of days to achieve enteral feeds of 150 mL/kg/d, and days of ventilation. Results are presented in Table 5. For each outcome measure, we show the overall significance level of the best model with the contributions made by its significant predictors. The ratio of number of predictor variables to number of subjects is rather high so caution in interpretation is advised.

Table 3 Mean (SD) VIQ, PIQ, and FSIQ scores for the whole group and for boys and girls separately
Table 4 Correlation coefficients (p levels) between %EBM and IQ measures for the whole group and boys and girls separately with level of maternal education and social class partialled out
Table 5 Results of regression analyses predicting IQ scores and neural volumes

%EBM and neural volumes.

We calculated partial correlations between %EBM and a series of neural volumes: total brain volume (TBV), left and right white matter volume (WMV) and left and right grey matter volume (GMV), adding test age/age at scan, and GA as covariates in addition to maternal education and social class, as above. Table 6 shows that %EBM was related significantly both to TBV and to left and right WMV in the whole group. The same was true in the boys, in whom the relationship between %EBM and WMV accounted for almost 50% of the variance. In girls, the relationship with WMV existed at a trend level only and that with TBV was not significant. No significant relationships existed between any %EBM and any GMV. Repeating the analyses using the additional covariates did not change these patterns of results except to reduce the size of the correlations between %EBM and WMV in girls, so that they were no longer significant even at a trend level. We repeated regression analyses as reported above, adding GA and age at test/scan as predictors. Results are shown in Table 5; the patterns for left and right GMV and WMV were the same so only the left results are reported.

Table 6 Correlations (p values) between %EBM and neural volumes for the whole group and boys and girls separately

The above analyses used absolute brain volumes. We repeated the WMV and GMV analyses covarying for TBV, i.e. using relative volumes (shown in Table 6). The results reflect the fact that these analyses deal with two component volumes contributing to TBV. Thus, although WMVs remained significantly related to %EBM, GMVs were now also significantly related but in the negative direction, the trend level relationships between %EBM and WMV in girls become significant. Finally, to further clarify the relative contributions of gray and white matter to TBV, we examined the correlations between %EBM and TBV. In all cases, covarying gray matter increased the size of the correlation coefficients while covarying white matter decreased them.

Neural volumes and IQ.

Finally, we examined the relationships between neural volumes and IQ scores (Table 7). For the whole group, left and right WMV correlated significantly with both VIQ and FSIQ. Boys showed these same relationships (with trend level relationships between PIQ and white matter) and, also, a significant correlation between TBV and VIQ. The nonsignificant relationships between IQ and neural volumes in girls and between IQ and gray matter in any group are omitted from Table 6.

Table 7 Significant correlations (p values) between neural volume measures and the three IQ scores for the whole group and for boys

Impact of donor BBM.

The diet group given banked donated breast milk as a supplement to EBM (n = 9) received 100% human milk from two sources. To check that the inclusion of this group had not unduly influenced the results, we eliminated the BBM group and analyzed data only from the 41 infants who had not received any donated human milk. The relationships between %EBM and VIQ and FSIQ in this smaller group were both significant at the p = 0.006 level. Regression analyses in these 41 subjects who received one of the other two supplements (i.e. TF or PTF) showed that neither supplement significantly predicted IQ outcome but %EBM did so for both VIQ and FSIQ. The effects of sole diets were not examined in this study. Similar results were found for neural volumes but are not reported here.

DISCUSSION

This study showed a dose-response relationship between early breast milk intake and later IQ and, uniquely, with whole brain volume at adolescence. In all subjects, but most clearly in boys, the effects of breast milk were seen more strongly on white than gray matter in the brain. These data support the hypothesis that one or more constituents of mothers' breast milk promote brain development at a structural level.

Our observation that IQ itself was strongly related to white matter, and hence TBV is consistent with the observed links between early breast milk feeding and both later IQ and later WMV. Relationships between white matter and measures of cognition have been reported previously in children (18). These data support the hypothesis that the beneficial impact of breastfeeding on IQ may be mediated, at least partly, by promoting the development of white matter.

Generalization of our findings to healthy full-term infants is untested but may be supported by the observations that breastfeeding in healthy infants is also related to higher subsequent IQ scores and that IQ is related to white matter in children and adults generally. In any case, preterm infants represent a sizeable proportion of the population (e.g. greater than the prevalence of type I diabetes), overrepresented among those with neurodevelopmental problems, so favorable influences on long-term brain development are important to identify even within this large subgroup.

Although correlations were seen in the whole population, they were always more highly significant in males. Thus, IQ was most clearly related to white matter and hence TBV in males, and expressed breast milk was linked to IQ and to neural volumes only in males. These findings concur with a large body of animal evidence; Smart (19) for example, examining the effect of early malnutrition on later learning and behavior in 165 animal studies, reported dominant effects in males. A growing literature in humans describes gender differences in brain development and structure (20,21) and in relationships between brain structure and cognition (22,23). We emphasize that males and females did not differ in mean IQ values but rather in the relationships between IQ and both diet and neural volumes. Percentage EBM accounted for around 25% of the variance in IQ in boys but only 5% in girls. It is interesting to note the recent report by Kesler et al. (24) that measures of WMV differ between preterm males and females.

Correlations were often found for FSIQ, but more detailed analysis showed that VIQ, rather than PIQ, was related to breastfeeding and neural volumes (except in one instance). There is growing evidence that early nutrition may have a selective, lasting effect on verbal subscales (8,25); in later development, however, diet may selectively affect PIQ (26). The implication is that the cognitive effects of nutritional intervention may depend on the timetable of brain development.

Possible mechanisms.

The mechanism for the observed effects is uncertain. In recent years, long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids (LCPUFAs), notably docosahexaenoic acid (DHA), have been considered a key explanation for neurodevelopmental benefits of breast milk. A recent meta-analysis of outcome in preterms after early LCPUFA supplementation of infant formula (27), however, found no conclusive evidence for long-term cognitive effects, suggesting that differences in early LCPUFA intake may not explain our observations. Although LCPUFAs remain of interest, it should be remembered that human milk contains a wide variety of other factors including hormones and growth factors, some of which (e.g. thyroxin, nerve growth factor) could plausibly influence cerebral development, although this is not established. During the window of brain development we studied, glial cell production and myelination are key factors related to volume increase (28). Our study was not designed to examine detailed neuronal development, but a plausible hypothesis for further testing is that breastfeeding promotes glial cell proliferation and, hence, myelination. Interestingly, a recent study showed that dietary manipulation in neonatal rats affected the expression of myelin basic protein more in males than in females (29), consistent with our findings of a greater effect of diet on white matter development in males.

Breast milk contains significant quantities of cholesterol, whereas infant formulas contain little (30); full-term breastfed infants have higher total serum cholesterol at 6 mo (31). Saher et al. (32) reported recently that cholesterol is an indispensable component of myelin membranes in mice and that cholesterol availability in oligodendrocytes is a rate-limiting factor in brain maturation. The massive increase in synapse number postnatally may require large amounts of cholesterol, obtained by neurons from astrocytes (33). Serum cholesterol has been related to intellectual performance in adults (34). We suggest, therefore, that dietary cholesterol intake could help explain an impact of breast milk on white matter development and cognition, through enhanced glial production/myelination.

Limitations.

We recognize limitations to the conclusions that can be drawn from this study because it did not have an experimental design, making it impossible to test for causation. We hope, however, to provide an impetus to further research and suggest to cognitive scientists that nutrition as an environmental factor may affect their outcomes. Testing for causation more formally in future intervention studies may be possible but, meanwhile, the strong observed dose-response relationships and the consistency of our findings with previous animal and human research makes causation plausible.

The study sample was restricted to preterm infants who had normal neurologic status at 7 y. We are unable to say anything about the relationships we have observed here in those preterms who show evidence of injury or adverse outcome. We do not know whether this group would show the same effects, smaller or larger. It is possible that %EBM might have a greater influence in this vulnerable group but this remains to be determined.

There are also some more specific limitations. Der et al. (4) have recently suggested that the relationship between breastfeeding and IQ is explained by the confounding influence of higher IQ in breastfeeding mothers. Although all mothers in this study had chosen to provide breast milk, we accept that residual confounding might remain, even within this subpopulation, if, for example, higher maternal IQ was associated with greater breast milk production. Lacking maternal IQ scores, we have used level of maternal education as a proxy measure and covaried this in all correlations. Although there was no significant relationship between %EBM and level of maternal education in the study population here, we accept that maternal IQ cannot be ruled out as a factor without directly measuring it. We have noted above that although the main outcome data, cognitive scores and neural volumes, were available for all subjects, data were missing for one control variable we used as a covariate, a weakness of the study.

During the age range studied here, gray matter and white matter develop along different trajectories (16). Neural volumes were obtained at one time point only which is why we have covaried age at scan in an attempt to crudely control for these differences. Repeated scans would have allowed observations about the effects of breast milk on the course of development. The results show that the %EBM ingested as a child is related to the volume of white matter seen in the brain at the age range studied here but the absolute volumes of white (and gray) matter might differ at different ages without affecting these correlation coefficients.

In summary, the present evidence supports the hypothesis that the effects of constituents of mothers' milk underlie the repeated observations that breastfeeding is associated with a higher cognitive level, at least in males. This has both scientific and clinical implications, pointing to nutritional practices that could optimize the attainment of cognitive potential. Our study also helps provide a framework for future mechanistic studies on the impact of early nutrition on brain development.