Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Brain Potentials of Conflict and Error-Likelihood Following Errorful and Errorless Learning in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder

  • Anke Hammer ,

    anke.hammer@ovgu.de

    Affiliation Department of Neuropsychology, Otto-von-Guericke University of Magdeburg, Magdeburg, Germany

  • Andreas Kordon,

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Luebeck, Luebeck, Germany

  • Marcus Heldmann,

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, Otto-von-Guericke University of Magdeburg, Magdeburg, Germany

  • Bartosz Zurowski,

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Luebeck, Luebeck, Germany, Neuroimage Nord, University of Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany

  • Thomas F. Münte

    Affiliations Department of Neuropsychology, Otto-von-Guericke University of Magdeburg, Magdeburg, Germany, Center for Behavioral Brain Sciences, Magdeburg, Germany

Abstract

Background

The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is thought to be overacting in patients with Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD) reflecting an enhanced action monitoring system. However, influences of conflict and error-likelihood have not been explored. Here, the error-related negativity (ERN) originating in ACC served as a measure of conflict and error-likelihood during memory recognition following different learning modes. Errorless learning prevents the generation of false memory candidates and has been shown to be superior to trial-and-error-learning. The latter, errorful learning, introduces false memory candidates which interfere with correct information in later recognition leading to enhanced conflict processing.

Methodology/Principal Findings

Sixteen OCD patients according to DSM-IV criteria and 16 closely matched healthy controls participated voluntarily in the event-related potential study. Both, OCD- and control group showed enhanced memory performance following errorless compared to errorful learning. Nevertheless, response-locked data showed clear modulations of the ERN amplitude. OCD patients compared to controls showed an increased error-likelihood effect after errorless learning. However, with increased conflict after errorful learning, OCD patients showed a reduced error-likelihood effect in contrast to controls who showed an increase.

Conclusion/Significance

The increase of the errorlikelihood effect for OCD patients within low conflict situations (recognition after errorless learning) might be conceptualized as a hyperactive monitoring system. However, within high conflict situations (recognition after EF-learning) the opposite effect was observed: whereas the control group showed an increased error-likelihood effect, the OCD group showed a reduction of the error-likelihood effect based on altered ACC learning rates in response to errors. These findings support theoretical frameworks explaining differences in ACC activity on the basis of conflict and perceived error-likelihood as influenced by individual error learning rate.

Introduction

In addition to other characteristic symptoms of obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD), such as chronic doubt, repetitive controlling, ruminations, and reduced behavioral flexibility [1], a number of neuropsychological studies have revealed altered memory functions [2][7] and executive dysfunction [8][14]. Following a systematic review [15], memory deficits are the most consistently reported neuropsychological features of OCD patients. A strategic memory deficit has been described for non-verbal material [7] and verbal material [16][19]. Other studies suggested that OCD patients have reduced confidence in the correctness of their memory contents, which consequently affects memory-based decisions [20][26]. These findings indicate that memory problems in OCD might be related to executive aspects of memory, such as meta-memory decisions about whether or not an associatively retrieved item has indeed been encountered before, rather than to deficits within the memory system proper. For example, OCD-patients might not have a problem in memorizing their shopping list per se but due to controlling or repetitive thinking about the memorized items the actual memory performance is impaired which might lead to incorrectly added and forgotten items.

Here, we tested this hypothesis by comparing two different encoding strategies between OCD patients and control participants. We used brain potentials to further delineate the characteristics of cognitive control during memory processes in OCD by means of errorless (henceforth EL) and errorful (henceforth EF) learning [27][30]. During EF-learning, interfering and thus possibly conflicting items are presented in addition to the relevant stimulus. In contrast, in EL-learning only the target stimulus is presented and only this stimulus without any further conflicting stimuli is thus available for storage resulting in an improved memory performance for EL learning as compared to EF learning. Baddeley and Wilson [27] assumed that the worse memory performance after EF-learning is due to the increased activation level of false candidates which leads to interference in recall. This interference is thought to be absent (or greatly diminished) in the EL modus as only one stimulus had been presented during learning. In EF-learning, memory impaired patients may not be able to use the remaining implicit memory resources, because they are not able to differentiate between errors made during learning and the correct information [27]. Consequently, these patients benefit from EL-learning compared to EF-learning as errors are avoided during the studying phase.

Rodriguez-Fornells and colleagues [29] and Heldmann et al. [31] investigated EL and EF-learning in conjunction with the recording of brain potentials. In both studies a word stem completion task was used. In EF learning, the initial three letters of a word (e.g. C-O-M) were presented and the participants were asked to guess which word the experimenter had in mind. After some guesses (e.g. compare, computer, commission, comedy) the experimenter indicated the correct word (e.g. comedy). This procedure introduces errors during learning as described above. In contrast, during EL learning, the intended word is given right after the initial letters and errors are prevented during learning. Brain potentials were acquired during recognition of words and the participants had to indicate via button response whether a word was learned before or not (correctly identifying target words or correctly rejecting non-target words). Both studies demonstrated EL/EF effects in particular for response-locked brain potentials, which are thought to reflect aspects related to the memory decision. In particular, a short latency midfrontal phasic negativity peaking at about 50 ms after the memory decision (here button press) was found to be modulated by learning mode. This negativity showed the topographic and latency characteristics of the error-related negativity (ERN) [32][35] previously described in research on action monitoring. It's neural source was consistently found in the posterior medial frontal cortex, most likely the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) as shown by brain potentials source localization studies [36][39] and error-related fMRI activity [40][42]; regions that are known to be involved in higher executive functions. Rodriguez-Fornells et al. [29] found the highest ERN amplitude for false alarms after EL-learning and intermediate-sized amplitudes for hits and false alarms in the EF condition. The smallest amplitude was found for hits in the EL condition and the ERN was absent for correct rejections. The modulation of the ERN amplitude in relation to memory decisions was interpreted as reflecting the activity of an internal monitoring device assessing the activation of the two possible decisions [29]. This interpretation places the occurrence of an ERN for memory decisions in the context of the conflict monitoring theory of the ERN [43] assuming that the activation depends on the product of current activations of concurrently available responses (here correct and guessed words). EF and EL learning was presented in an intermixed procedure which might lead to a rudimentary activation of non-targets in EL learning as well. In a further study in healthy participants, Heldmann et al. [31] presented EF and EL learning in blocked sessions and included additional new words during recognition which did not occur during learning. Thus there were more words that needed to be rejected (non-targets require a NO response) as compared to words to be recognized (targets require a YES response) resulting in an unequal ratio of NO and YES responses. In other words, the risk to make an error is increased for YES as compared to NO responses. Irrespective of the correctness of the response, Heldmann et al. [31] observed an ERN for items classified as learned before (i.e. YES-responses: hits and false alarms) as compared to items classified as not learned before (i.e. NO-responses: correct rejections and misses). These results lead to the argument that variations of the ERN amplitude in EL/EF-learning might be partially explained by the subjects' perceived likelihood of making an error. This interpretation was based on the error-likelihood model [44], [45], [but see also 46], which postulates that the activation of the ACC (and thus its electrophysiological counterpart, the ERN) is not modulated by the presence of conflict or the detection of an error per se but the perceived probability of making an error (here the ratio of YES/NO responses).

There are alternative theoretical approaches explaining modulations of the ERN: the error detection approach [33] and its extension, the reinforcement learning model [47]. Following the reinforcement model an error is understood as a negative reinforcement signal processed within the mesencephalic dopaminergic system. The resulting changes in dopaminergic activity are used for further adaptation of behavior in order to avoid errors in the future. For the given investigation we would like to focus on the error likelihood model for two reasons. First, this model was a good candidate explaining the findings of Heldmann et al. [31] and second, an extension of the model takes individual differences into account. This extension of the error-likelihood model [48], [49] showed that the model can account for individual differences related to error-likelihood, prediction of error consequences, and conflict effects in ACC. Individuals with a high learning rate within ACC (i.e. learning from errors, adapting the following behavior to circumvent unwanted consequences in future) resemble the known patterns of the model: the higher the probability to commit an error the higher the activity within ACC. In contrast, individuals with a slow learning rate and thus a reduced ability within ACC to learn from errors showed smaller error-likelihood effects whereas conflict effects increased. This finding indicated an inverse relationship between conflict and error-likelihood effects dependent on the error learning rate [48], [49]. Such a result is of specific interest for OCD patients as previous results suggested an impaired cognitive control and thus possibly altered learning rates in ACC following errors. Previous studies in OCD patients found increased amplitudes of the ERN [50][56], [see 57 for partly inconsistent results], which has been interpreted as evidence for an increased action monitoring compared to controls. This interpretation has been corroborated by neuroimaging data showing hyperactivity of the ACC in OCD patients which was positively correlated with symptom severity [58], [59].

None of these studies modulated the perceived error-likelihood via different learning modes. We used brain potentials to assess executive aspects of memory in OCD by contrasting EF and EL-learning. OCD-patients are continuously monitoring their behaviour but still remain with the feeling of erroneous actions and states [60]. The present paradigm is of specific interest because (a) we can compare errors in conflicting (EF) with conflict-reduced circumstances (EL) and additionally (b) correct responses in differential conflicting situations. These differences in conflict processing can be evaluated based on different learning modes, i.e. recognition following errorful learning as compared to errorless learning is thought to be conflicting based on the additional interfering material. Here, influences of error-likelihood can be evaluated on the basis of the response options (Yes vs. No response) depending on the ratio of target and non-target items (see also [31]).

In healthy subjects, we expected to replicate the basic findings for EL and EF-learning [29], [31]. However, in OCD patients we expected an increased ERN for EL-learning compared to a control group [50][55], [57]. EF-learning increases the interference in later recognition – and thus the action monitoring system is challenged in particular. The present study was designed to answer the question which one of the following is true for OCD patients: Either the dysfunctional action monitoring system in OCD patients (1) is overactive resulting in increased ERN amplitudes for all stimuli [50][55], [57] or (2) show different error-likelihood effects (i.e. increased following EL and decreased following EF-learning) as postulated by the error-likelihood model for individuals with altered error learning rates [48], [49].

Materials and Methods

The study was approved by the ethical committees of the Universities of Magdeburg and Lübeck.

Participants

Sixteen German-speaking adults (six women, mean age 37.0) with the diagnosis of OCD as defined by DSM-IV criteria [61] and 16 neurologically and psychiatrically healthy control participants (six women, mean age 36.7) matched for age, school education and handedness participated after giving written informed consent. Table 1 gives the demographic and clinical characteristics of both groups.

thumbnail
Table 1. Detailed group characteristics of the obsessive-compulsive disorder group and the control group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0006553.t001

Experimental procedure

Subjects participated in one EF-learning and one EL-learning session. The order of learning sessions was counterbalanced across subjects. One session comprised 6 runs each composed of a learning phase and a subsequent recognition phase. Each participant performed a word-fragment-completion task for 20 word-fragments [29]. In the EF condition, the first three letters of a word were given by the experimenter and the subject was asked to guess words to complete this fragment. The following example instructions were given by the experimenter: “I am thinking of a word that begins with the letters B-R-U”. The participant could have been guessed “Bruder” (brother). After the first answer the participant was required to have another guess, for example “Brust” (chest). After guessing some words (usually around 2–3 words), the experimenter revealed which word was the target word to be remembered. If subjects failed to guess the intended target word, the experimenter introduced example words and the target word. For each of the presented word-fragments at least two German words exist with a high and comparable guessing probability (German stem completion study, data courtesy Richardson-Klavehn and Düzel, unpublished), e.g. BRU: ‘Bruder’ [brother], ‘Brust’ [chest] [29]. Both of these words were produced with 34% probability in the German stem completion study. This triplet could have been competed with other but lower probability candidates, e.g. “Brunnen” (well, 13%), “Brunst” (ardour, 6%) or “Brutal” (brutal, 3%) [see also 29]. However, these words were not used during the recognition phase. For each fragment one high probability word was used during the learning phase as a target word, while another high probability alternative was used as distracter during the recognition phase. In the EL-learning condition the first three letters of the word were introduced by the experimenter directly followed by the target word. The EL trial was introduced as in the following example: “I am thinking of a word that begins with A–N–Z. This word is ‘Anzeige’ (advertisement, 53%)”. The subject had to repeat the target word immediately without guessing any additional words. Next to “Anzeige”, “Anzahl” (number, 28%) could have been another high probability word. This word was presented in the recognition phase as a non-target word for half of the subjects. The other half of the subjects had “Anzahl” as target in the EL condition with “Anzeige” being used as non-target during recognition. An example of stimuli assignment to the learning mode sessions per list is given in Table 2 [see also 29].

During EF-learning the participants guessed several words to complete the word-fragment which resulted in deeper processing of words as compared to errorless word list learning. To ensure such a deeper processing of words in the EL condition as well, participants had to produce a sentence with the word.

During each recognition phase, 20 targets, 20 distracters and 20 additional new words were presented in a randomized order [see also 31]. The task was to indicate by button press (right index/middle finger), whether or not a given word was a target word. The participants did not receive feedback about the correctness of the actual response. The words were presented in white letters on a black background in the middle of a computer screen. Stimuli subtended 0.57° in height and between 1.7° and 4.9° in width. The stimulus duration was 300 ms with a stimulus-onset-asynchrony between 1800 and 2500 ms.

EEG recording and analysis

Electroencephalography and electrooculography signals were registered with a digitization rate of 250 Hz and filtered with a bandpass of 0.01–30 Hz. Twenty-nine tin electrodes mounted in an elastic cap were positioned according to the 10/20 system (Fp1/2, F3/4, C3/4, P3/4, O1/O2, F7/8, T7/8, P7/8, Fc1/2, Cp1/2, Po3/Po4, Fc5/6, Cp5/6, Fz, Cz, Pz). Bio-signals were re-referenced offline to the mean activity of two electrodes placed on the right and left mastoid. Eye movements were recorded in order to allow for later offline rejection. All electrode impedances were kept below 5 kΩ. Using individualized amplitude criteria on the electrooculography, trials with eye movement artifacts were excluded from further analysis. Response-locked brain potentials were averaged for epochs of 900 ms length with 300 ms baseline. The combination of learning mode (EF/EL), stimulus type (target, non-target, new word) and response (correct/incorrect) resulted in 12 different trial types. Because the frequency of false alarms for new words and false alarms for the EL modus was too low, these categories had to be neglected in the analysis (Table 3 for remaining trial types). The statistical analysis was performed using repeated measures designs as specified in the Results section. The target component for the evaluation of the brain potentials was the ERN. To evaluate the ERN repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted including the between-subjects factor Group (OCD vs. Control) and within subjects factors Learning Mode (EF vs. EL-learning), Response-type (hit, miss, new correct rejection), Electrode site (Fz, Cz) as within subject factors. This overall analysis was followed by more detailed ANOVAs as specified within the Result section. In order to evaluate the effect of error-likelihood an additional response factor (YES vs. NO responses) was included if applicable. Response-locked brain potentials were filtered with a 1–8 Hz bandpass filter prior to analysis. Mean amplitudes were calculated in the time window 0–100 ms (baseline −300 to 0) after response and entered into analyses of variance. For all statistical effects involving two or more degrees of freedom in the numerator, the Greenhouse-Geisser epsilon procedure was used to correct for possible violations of the sphericity assumption. Additionally, tests involving electrode x condition interactions were carried out on normalized data using the vector normalization procedure [62], [63]. Planned comparisons were calculated testing for differences between hits, false alarms and correct rejection within each learning condition as well as for response category differences between learning conditions.

Results

Performance measures

Signal detection measures revealed that memory accuracy was significantly better for EL compared to EF-learning in both groups (Table 4). A Group (OCD vs. control) by Learning Mode (EF vs. EL) ANOVA on the signal detection measure d' revealed a clear main effect of Learning Mode (F(1,30) = 379, p<.0001), whereas neither the Group effect (F(1,30) = 0.23, not significant (ns)) nor the interaction Group by Learning Mode reached significance. The same pattern emerged for the measures beta (Group: F(1,30) = 0.84, ns; Learning Mode: F(1,30) = 7.24, p<.05; Group x Learning Mode: F(1,30) = 2.43, ns) and criterion (Group: F(1,30) = 0.08, ns; Learning Mode: F(1,30) = 263, p<.001; Group x Learning Mode: F(1,30) = 2.84, ns). Thus, both groups showed better performance measures for the EL learning compared to EF learning and no clear differences could be found between groups.

The new words did not appear in the learning phase before and thus, should not be influenced by learning mode. Indeed, no significant effects were obtained for reaction times for new word correct rejections (ANOVA: Group: F(1,30) = 2.34, ns; Learning Mode: F(1,30) = 0.64, ns; Group x Learning Mode: F(1,30) = 0.61, ns), indicating no influence of learning mode on new words and no differences between both groups in terms of reaction times. However, reaction times for hits showed a significant main effect for Learning Mode (F(1,30) = 6.38, p<.05) but no Group (F(1,30) = 2.85, ns) or interaction effect (F(1,30) = 0.01, ns). In the pair-wise comparisons (Table 4) the Learning Mode effect on reaction times was only significant for the control group with faster reactions to EL compared to EF stimuli but no statistical difference was found for the OCD patients. For reaction times to misses both main effects did not reach significance (Group: F(1,30) = 2.37, ns; Learning Mode: F(1,30) = 3.25, ns) but we found a significant interaction (Group x Learning Mode: F(1,30) = 4.71, p<.05). Direct comparisons revealed that OCD-patients were slower in EF compared to EL trials but reaction times of the control group were similar for EL and EF trials (see Table 4). No differences were found for reaction times to false alarms (Group: F(1,30) = 1.19, ns; Learning Mode: F(1,30) = 2.46, ns; Group x Learning Mode: F(1,30) = 0.79, ns). Finally, reaction times to correct rejection yielded a main effect of Group (F(1,30) = 4.94, p<.05) and Learning Mode (F(1,30) = 11.39, p<.01) but no significant interaction. Tracing these effects by pair-wise comparisons showed that both groups showed faster reaction times for EL trials but that the control group showed overall faster responses (Table 4). Thus, reaction times to neutral stimuli (i.e. new words) were similar for both learning modes and both groups. However, responses to EL stimuli were faster as compared to EF trials for false alarms in both groups. The OCD patients showed faster responses to EL misses as compared to EF misses and no differences for hits whereas the control groups showed the opposite pattern, i.e. similar response times for misses in EL and EF learning and faster responses for EL hits as compared to EF hits.

Response-locked brain potentials

Both groups showed a fronto-central negativity, which was most prominent for hits after EL and EF-learning as compared to new correct rejections and misses (Figure 1, for corresponding topographical maps see Figure 2 and for mean amplitudes see Figure 3). The distribution of this component suggests that it is an instance of the ERN. The overall ANOVA revealed consistent main effects for Learning Mode (F(1,30) = 35.43, p<.001) and Response-type (F(1,30) = 4.94, p<.05) and a Learning Mode x Response-type interaction (F(1,30) = 31.41, p<.001). The remaining effects did not reach significance.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Response-locked potentials for OCD- and control-group.

Response-locked ERPs (negativity is plotted up and each hash mark represents 100 ms of activity in this and in the following figures) of OCD patients (upper panel, N = 16) and control group (lower panel, N = 16). Hits related to both learning conditions and errorful false alarms result in an increased negativity compared to misses and both correct rejections. For the errorless mode (left panel) this is enhanced for OCD as compared to control group. For the errorful condition the opposite is true (most prominent at Fz).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0006553.g001

thumbnail
Figure 2. Topographical distributions of the brain potentials.

Spline-interpolated isovoltage maps at 60 ms reveal a fronto-central distribution of the brain potentials. Darkest color is most negative.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0006553.g002

thumbnail
Figure 3. Mean amplitudes of the ERN.

Bar graphs of mean ERP amplitudes at electrode sites Fz and Cz (0–100 ms after response) for the control groups (left) and OCD group (right).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0006553.g003

Subsequently, ANOVAs were computed separately for the two learning modes (Group x Response-type x electrode site). In OCD-patients, the EL hits resulted in the largest ERN as compared to the other responses. This difference was not as pronounced for the control group (Figure 1, left panel). These findings were corroborated in a significant main effect for Group (F(1,30) = 9.3, p<.01) and a Group x Response-type (F(2,60) = 14.66, p<.001) interaction (Response-type (F(2,60) = 2.86, ns.). For the EF learning, an opposite direction was observed between groups: Here the EF hits resulted in an increased ERN within both groups but were enlarged for the control group compared to the OCD group (Figure 1, right panel). Statistically this was confirmed by the analogous ANOVA for the EF condition, which revealed a significant main effect for Group (F(1,30) = 6.64, p<.05), Response-type (F(2,60) = 5.5, p<.01) as well as an interaction between these two factors (F(2,60) = 29.67, p<.001). These results show a clear differentiation between both groups for both learning modes.

EF-learning resulted in a sufficient number of false alarms. In both groups, EF false alarms were associated with an enlarged ERN response (right panel of Figure 1and Figure 4), which appeared to be smaller in the OCD group. An additional ANOVA including false alarms was performed with the factors Group and Response-type (2 levels: correct [hit, correct rejection] vs. erroneous [miss, false alarms]), Response (2 levels: yes [hit, false alarm] vs. no [miss, correct rejection]) and electrode site (Fz, Cz). Visual inspection suggested an increased negativity for yes responses compared to no responses in particular within the control group (see Figure 1 right panel, and Figure 3). Statistically, this was corroborated by a significant main effect for Response (F(1,30) = 91.37, p<.001) and a significant interaction between Group and Response (F(1,30) = 21.04, p<.001). All other effects were not significant (all df 1,30, all F<2.2). Planned pair-wise comparisons within groups (all df 1,15) were performed to trace back amplitude differences. Comparing the ERN amplitude of EL hits and misses we found a significant difference for the OCD group (F = 6.53, p<.05) but not for the control group (F = 1.93, p>.05). For EF-learning we observed the opposite: there was no significant difference for the OCD-group for EF hits vs. misses (F = 0.30, ns) and EF false alarm vs. correct rejection (F = 0.19, ns) but a significant difference for the control group (EF hit vs. misses: F = 5.28, p<.05, EF false alarm vs. correct rejection: F = 7.69, p<.05). Figure 4 illustrates this pattern: Whereas a clear differentiation between false alarm and new correct rejection was observed for the control group, this was absent for the OCD-group. Directly comparing the response types between both conditions (EL hit vs. EF hit, EL miss vs. EF miss, EL correct rejection vs. EF correct rejection, EL new correct rejection vs. EF new correct rejection) did not show significant differences (all F<1.45, p>.2).

thumbnail
Figure 4. Difference waves of false alarms and correct rejections.

Response-locked ERPs of false alarms in comparison to new correct rejection for the control (left panel) and OCD group (right panel). The grey line shows the difference wave of false alarm minus new correct rejections. The corresponding spline-interpolated isovoltage maps of the difference wave shows a fronto-central distribution for the control group. This effect is nearly absent for the OCD group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0006553.g004

Discussion

OCD-patients and the healthy control group benefited from EL-learning as compared to EF-learning indicated by improved memory performance which is in accord with earlier studies showing enhanced memory performance for EL-learning [27], [29]. Contrary to our expectations, OCD patients as compared to the controls did neither show a decreased memory performance following EF-learning nor an enhanced memory performance following EL-learning. However, we found clear differential modulations of an early negativity obtained time-locked to the response in both groups. In line with previous electrophysiological investigations [29], [31], [64] we identified this negativity as an ERN based on its polarity, latency and topographical distribution (see Figures 1 and 2). There were significant differences between the two groups for the ERN to false alarm-trials in EF-learning and for hits from both, EL and EF conditions which hint at differences in the executive control of memory between OCD and control participants. In line with Heldmann et al. [31], we expected increased amplitudes for items with a high likelihood to commit an error following the postulations of the error-likelihood model [44], [46], [48], [49]. According to the error-likelihood model, it is not primarily the conflict or the error-detection that causes the activity in ACC observed as an ERN but rather the perceived probability of committing an error. The participants performed a word-list recognition task following EF- and EL-learning mode. In either case, the participant had to respond with YES if a word was recognized as a learned word and NO if it was recognized as a new word or distracter word. During the recognition phase there were twice as many non-target words (i.e. distracter words beginning with the same three letters and totally new words) than target words (i.e. learned words). In case of perfect recognition, the ratio of target words (requested yes-response) and non-target words (requested no response) was 1∶2 [see also 31]. The likelihood to commit an error with a YES-response (hit, false alarm) was twice as high as compared to a NO-response (correct rejection, miss). Heldmann and colleagues [31] found major differences of the ERN amplitude between yes- and no-responses. The error-likelihood model would predict such a difference: an increased ACC activity would be expected for all yes-responses compared to no-responses regardless of the correctness of response (Hit/false alarm) and the learning mode. Turning to the present data, this prediction from the error-likelihood model was borne out with yes responses from both learning modes associated with an increased negativity generally in both groups (see Figure 1).

However, there were marked differences between OCD patients and control subjects as illustrated by the bar graphs in Figure 3. The EL session resembles a standard wordlist recognition task. Here, the ERN to hits was enhanced in the OCD group compared to the control group. This result is in line with earlier reports of an enhanced ERN amplitude for OCD patients [50][57] and the hyperactivity of the ACC for OCD patients as shown by neuroimaging data [58], [59]. The amplitude enhancement has been interpreted as reflecting an overactive action monitoring system in OCD, an interpretation that is substantiated by an increased post-error slowing [55]. This interpretation also squares with the view that OCD is associated with a dysbalanced activity within cortical-striatal-thalamic-cortical circuits [65][69].

However, this effect was different following EF-learning which cannot simply be explained by the error-likelihood model. As outlined in the introduction, the guessed words during the learning phase might interfere with the learned words and produce a conflict in later recognition. The conflict monitoring theory [43], [70], [71] proposes that the ERN may reflect the degree of a response conflict between multiple response alternatives. Conflicting responses evoke a situation when errors are likely to be committed. Thus, following EF-learning there might be a “double impact” on ERN amplitude: an increased error-likelihood for yes responses and an increased conflict due to interfering false candidates after EF-learning. This leads to increased ERN amplitude for EF false alarms and hits compared to correct rejections for the control group (Figure 1 and 3). Intuitively one would expect a similar (if not even more pronounced) effect for OCD patients because of the frontal hyperactivity, specifically in ACC. However, for OCD-patients the opposite is the case. While OCD patients showed the largest ERN difference following the EL-modus, this difference is diminished after EF-learning (see Figure 4 for a direct comparison of EF false alarm and new correct rejection).

Brown and Braver extended their model introducing individual differences based on different learning capabilities attributed to the ACC [48], [49]. In the following, we discuss our own results in the light of this extension. Here both, ‘YES’ and ‘NO’ responses resulted in a pattern as predicted by the error-likelihood model [44]. However, the influence of EF-learning had different impacts on both groups. Focusing on ‘YES’ responses, we observed increasing ERN amplitudes as the impact of conflict increases for the control group (lowest for EL hit and highest for EF false alarm). For the OCD-group the opposite picture emerged (highest for EL hit and lowest for EF false alarm, see Figure 1 and 4). This appears to be at odds with the notion of a hyperactive monitoring system in OCD patients which might lead us to expect increased ACC activity with a double impact of increased perceived likelihood and increased conflict. Brown and Braver's extended error-likelihood model [45], [49] is able to resolve these counterintuitive results: Individuals with altered ACC function (i.e. slow learning rate) showed reduced error-likelihood effects whereas response conflict was increased and vice versa for not-altered ACC functioning (i.e. fast learning rate). Thus, fast learning rates increase the efficiency of learning from errors, which increases the ability to predict an error at the expense of response conflict. The response conflict in our study consisted of two related effects: increased error-likelihood and increased number of possible responses. For the given design, the error-likelihood was constant over EL- and EF-learning as the ratio of YES- and NO-responses was the same for both learning modes. However, the amount of possible responses (here the amount of activated words depending on learning mode) differed: EF-learning introduced two alternative incorrect candidates which intervene in later recognition increasing response conflict compared to EL-learning (please note, however, that during recognition there might be still reduced conflict compared to EF based on incorrect memory traces). Figure 5 shows the error-likelihood effects for both groups (correct ‘YES’ responses (hits) minus correct ‘NO’ responses (correct rejection)) depending on the learning mode. None of the groups showed an absent or diminished error-likelihood effect as predicted by the model, which might be due to an increased cognitive load of the word list experiment as compared to the stop-and-change paradigm used by Brown and Braver (i.e. EL-learning is not purely conflict free but significantly less conflicting compared to EF).

thumbnail
Figure 5. Error-likelihood effects.

Error-likelihood effects of OCD and control participants. Bar graphs of the mean amplitude difference of correct Yes responses (hits) and No responses (correct rejections) at electrode sites Fz and Cz (0–100 ms) for EL-learning (reduced conflict) and EF-learning (high conflict).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0006553.g005

However, the model is a good candidate to explain the effects for the EF modus. Following EF-learning, the OCD group showed a reduced error-likelihood effect as compared to the control-group (see Figure 5, right panel). This result was predicted by the extended error-likelihood model of Brown and Braver [48], [49] for altered ACC-functioning: slow ACC learning rates resulted in a decreased error-likelihood effect. Within a low conflict situation (recognition after EL-learning) OCD-patients compared to the control group show a considerable increase of the error-likelihood effect. This might be conceptualized as a hyperactive monitoring system [50][55], [57]. For high conflict situations (recognition after EF-learning) the opposite effect was observed: whereas the control group showed an increased error-likelihood effect, the OCD group showed a considerable reduction of the error-likelihood effect based on altered ACC learning rates in response to errors [48], [49]. This interpretation is supported by reports that OCD-patients compared to controls showed increased decision difficulties for simple or less risky situation (e.g. “seeing a piece of string on the ground”), whereas no differences were found for difficult or high risky decisions (e.g. “seeing a sharp wire in the parking lot”) [26] and might explain why OCD patients show decision difficulties in daily life for simple situations (‘Indecisiveness’ e.g. which detergent should be bought).

In conclusion, EL-learning enhances memory performance compared to trial-and-error learning (EF modus) in both groups. Differential ERN patterns of OCD patients and the healthy control group support the view of an altered conflict monitoring system and perceived error-likelihood effects in OCD.

Acknowledgments

We thank Antoni Rodriguez-Fornells (Institució Catalana de Recerca i Estudis Avanc, ats (ICREA), Barcelona, Spain) for his input in earlier studies on errorless learning. Specifically, we thank the patients and the control group members for their participation.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: AH MH TFM. Performed the experiments: AH. Analyzed the data: AH AK BZ. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: AH AK MH BZ TFM. Wrote the paper: AH TFM. Critical revision for important intellectual content of the paper: AK MH BZ.

References

  1. 1. Savage CR, Rauch SL (2000) Cognitive deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 157: 1182–1183.
  2. 2. Boone KB, Ananth J, Phipott L, Kaur A, Djerenderedjian A (1991) Neuropsychological characteristics of nondepressed adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychiatr Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 4: 96–109.
  3. 3. Christensen KJ, Kim SW, Dysken MW, Hoover KM (1992) Neuropsychological performance in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 31: 4–18.
  4. 4. Cohen LJ, Hollander E, DeCaria CM, Stein DJ, Simeon D, et al. (1996) Specificity of neuropsychological impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a comparison with social phobic and normal control subjects. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 8: 82–85.
  5. 5. Dirson S, Bouvard M, Cottraux J, Martin R (1995) Visual memory impairment in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder: a controlled study. Psychother Psychosom 63: 22–31.
  6. 6. Savage CR, Keuthen NJ, Jenike MA, Brown HD, Baer L, et al. (1996) Recall and recognition memory in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 8: 99–103.
  7. 7. Savage CR, Baer L, Keuthen NJ, Brown HD, Rauch SL, et al. (1999) Organizational strategies mediate nonverbal memory impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 45: 905–916.
  8. 8. Abbruzzese M, Ferri S, Scarone S (1997) The selective breakdown of frontal functions in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and in patients with schizophrenia: a double dissociation experimental finding. Neuropsychologia 35: 907–912.
  9. 9. Abbruzzese M, Ferri S, Scarone S (1995) Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in obsessive-compulsive disorder: no evidence for involvement of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Psychiatry Res 58: 37–43.
  10. 10. Abbruzzese M, Bellodi L, Ferri S, Scarone S (1995) Frontal lobe dysfunction in schizophrenia and obsessive-compulsive disorder: a neuropsychological study. Brain Cogn 27: 202–212.
  11. 11. Malloy P (1987) Frontal lobe dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder. In: Perecman E, editor. The frontal lobes revisited. New York: IRBN Press. pp. 207–223.
  12. 12. Purcell R, Maruff P, Kyrios M, Pantelis C (1998) Neuropsychological deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a comparison with unipolar depression, panic disorder, and normal controls. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55: 415–423.
  13. 13. Purcell R, Maruff P, Kyrios M, Pantelis C (1998) Cognitive deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder on tests of frontal-striatal function. Biol Psychiatry 43: 348–357.
  14. 14. Veale DM, Sahakian BJ, Owen AM, Marks IM (1996) Specific cognitive deficits in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychol Med 26: 1261–1269.
  15. 15. Kuelz AK, Hohagen F, Voderholzer U (2004) Neuropsychological performance in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a critical review. Biol Psychol 65: 185–236.
  16. 16. Cabrera AR, McNally RJ, Savage CR (2001) Missing the forest for the trees? Deficient memory for linguistic gist in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychol Med 31: 1089–1094.
  17. 17. Deckersbach T, Savage CR, Phillips KA, Wilhelm S, Buhlmann U, et al. (2000) Characteristics of memory dysfunction in body dysmorphic disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 6: 673–681.
  18. 18. Henin A, Savage CR, Rauch SL, Deckersbach T, Wilhelm S, et al. (2001) Is age at symptom onset associated with severity of memory impairment in adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder? Am J Psychiatry 158: 137–139.
  19. 19. Savage CR, Deckersbach T, Wilhelm S, Rauch SL, Baer L, et al. (2000) Strategic processing and episodic memory impairment in obsessive compulsive disorder. Neuropsychology 14: 141–151.
  20. 20. Foa EB, Amir N, Gershuny B, Molnar C, Kozak MJ (1997) Implicit and explicit memory in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Anxiety Disord 11: 119–129.
  21. 21. Hermans D, Engelen U, Grouwels L, Joos E, Lemmens J, et al. (2008) Cognitive confidence in obsessive-compulsive disorder: distrusting perception, attention and memory. Behav Res Ther 46: 98–113.
  22. 22. MacDonald PA, Antony MM, Macleod CM, Richter MA (1997) Memory and confidence in memory judgements among individuals with obsessive compulsive disorder and non-clinical controls. Behav Res Ther 35: 497–505.
  23. 23. Radomsky AS, Rachman S, Hammond D (2001) Memory bias, confidence and responsibility in compulsive checking. Behav Res Ther 39: 813–822.
  24. 24. Radomsky AS, Rachman S (1999) Memory bias in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Behav Res Ther 37: 605–618.
  25. 25. Rubenstein CS, Peynircioglu ZF, Chambless DL, Pigott TA (1993) Memory in sub-clinical obsessive-compulsive checkers. Behav Res Ther 31: 759–765.
  26. 26. Tolin DF, Abramowitz JS, Brigidi BD, Amir N, Street GP, et al. (2001) Memory and memory confidence in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behav Res Ther 39: 913–927.
  27. 27. Baddeley A, Wilson BA (1994) When implicit learning fails: amnesia and the problem of error elimination. Neuropsychologia 32: 53–68.
  28. 28. Wilson BA, Baddeley A, Evans J, Shiel A (1994) Errorless learning in the rehabilitation of memory impaired people. Neuropsychol Rehab 4: 307–326.
  29. 29. Rodriguez-Fornells A, Kofidis C, Munte TF (2004) An electrophysiological study of errorless learning. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 19: 160–173.
  30. 30. Terrace HS (1963) Discrimination learning with and without “errors”. J Exp Anal Behav 6: 1–27.
  31. 31. Heldmann M, Markgraf U, Rodriguez-Fornells A, Munte TF (2008) Brain potentials reveal the role of conflict in human errorful and errorless learning. Neurosci Lett.
  32. 32. Gehring WJ, Goss B, Coles MGH, Meyer DE, Donchin E (1993) A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science 4: 385–390.
  33. 33. Falkenstein M, Hohnsbein J, Hoormann J, Blanke L (1991) Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 78: 447–455.
  34. 34. Holroyd CB, Nieuwenhuis S, Yeung N, Cohen JD (2003) Errors in reward prediction are reflected in the event-related brain potential. Neuroreport 14: 2481–2484.
  35. 35. Holroyd CB, Yeung N (2003) Alcohol and error processing. Trends Neurosci 26: 402–404.
  36. 36. Dehaene S, Posner MI, Tucker DM (1994) Localization of a neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science 5: 303–305.
  37. 37. Luu P, Tucker DM (2001) Regulating action: alternating activation of midline frontal and motor cortical networks. Clinical Neurophysiology 112: 1295–1306.
  38. 38. Herrmann M, Rotte M, Grubich C, Ebert AD, Schiltz K, et al. (2001) Control of semantic interference in episodic memory retrieval is associated with an anterior cingulate-prefrontal activation pattern. Hum Brain Mapp 13: 94–103.
  39. 39. van Veen V, Holroyd CB, Cohen JD, Stenger VA, Carter CS (2004) Errors without conflict: implications for performance monitoring theories of anterior cingulate cortex. Brain Cogn 56: 267–276.
  40. 40. Ullsperger M, von Cramon DY (2004) Decision making, performance and outcome monitoring in frontal cortical areas. Nat Neurosci 7: 1173–1174.
  41. 41. Ullsperger M, von Cramon DY (2001) Subprocesses of performance monitoring: a dissociation of error processing and response competition revealed by event-related fMRI and ERPs. Neuroimage 14: 1387–1401.
  42. 42. Ridderinkhof KR, Ullsperger M, Crone EA, Nieuwenhuis S (2004) The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science 306: 443–447.
  43. 43. Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, Cohen JD (2001) Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol Rev 108: 624–652.
  44. 44. Brown JW, Braver TS (2005) Learned predictions of error likelihood in the anterior cingulate cortex. Science 307: 1118–1121.
  45. 45. Brown JW, Reynolds JR, Braver TS (2007) A computational model of fractionated conflict-control mechanisms in task-switching. Cognit Psychol 55: 37–85.
  46. 46. Nieuwenhuis S, Schweizer TS, Mars RB, Botvinick MM, Hajcak G (2007) Error-likelihood prediction in the medial frontal cortex: a critical evaluation. Cereb Cortex 17: 1570–1581.
  47. 47. Holroyd CB, Coles MG (2002) The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev 109: 679–709.
  48. 48. Brown JW, Braver TS (2007) Risk prediction and aversion by anterior cingulate cortex. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 7: 266–277.
  49. 49. Brown JW, Braver TS (2008) A computational model of risk, conflict, and individual difference effects in the anterior cingulate cortex. Brain Res 1202: 99–108.
  50. 50. Johannes S, Wieringa BM, Nager W, Rada D, Dengler R, et al. (2001) Discrepant target detection and action monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res 108: 101–110.
  51. 51. Johannes S, Wieringa BM, Mantey M, Nager W, Rada D, et al. (2001) Altered inhibition of motor responses in Tourette Syndrome and Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Acta Neurol Scand 104: 36–43.
  52. 52. Gehring WJ, Himle J, Nisenson LG (2000) Action-monitoring dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychol Sci 11: 1–6.
  53. 53. Hajcak G, Simons RF (2002) Error-related brain activity in obsessive-compulsive undergraduates. Psychiatry Res 110: 63–72.
  54. 54. Santesso DL, Segalowitz SJ, Schmidt LA (2006) Error-related electrocortical responses are enhanced in children with obsessive-compulsive behaviors. Dev Neuropsychol 29: 431–445.
  55. 55. Münte T, Heldmann M, Hinrichs H, Marco-Pallares M, Krämer U, et al. (2008) Nucleus accumbens is involved in human action monitoring: Evidence from invasive electrophysiological recordings. Front Hum Neurosci 1:
  56. 56. Endrass T, Klawohn J, Schuster F, Kathmann N (2008) Overactive performance monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder: ERP evidence from correct and erroneous reactions. Neuropsychologia 46: 1877–1887.
  57. 57. Nieuwenhuis S, Nielen MM, Mol N, Hajcak G, Veltman DJ (2005) Performance monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res 134: 111–122.
  58. 58. Fitzgerald KD, Welsh RC, Gehring WJ, Abelson JL, Himle JA, et al. (2005) Error-related hyperactivity of the anterior cingulate cortex in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 57: 287–294.
  59. 59. Ursu S, Stenger VA, Shear MK, Jones MR, Carter CS (2003) Overactive action monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Psychol Sci 14: 347–353.
  60. 60. Pitman RK (1987) A cybernetic model of obsessive-compulsive psychopathology. Compr Psychiatry 28: 334–343.
  61. 61. American-Psychiatric-Association (1994) DSM-IV: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorder. Washington, DC.: American Psychiatric Association.
  62. 62. McCarthy G, Wood CC (1985) Scalp distributions of event-related potentials: an ambiguity associated with analysis of variance models. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 62: 203–208.
  63. 63. Urbach TP, Kutas M (2002) The intractability of scaling scalp distributions to infer neuroelectric sources. Psychophysiology 39: 791–808.
  64. 64. Nessler D, Mecklinger A (2003) ERP correlates of true and false recognition after different retention delays: stimulus- and response-related processes. Psychophysiology 40: 146–159.
  65. 65. den Braber A, Ent DV, Blokland GA, van Grootheest DS, Cath DC, et al. (2008) An fMRI study in monozygotic twins discordant for obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Biol Psychol.
  66. 66. Kopell BH, Greenberg B, Rezai AR (2004) Deep brain stimulation for psychiatric disorders. J Clin Neurophysiol 21: 51–67.
  67. 67. Rauch SL, Whalen PJ, Curran T, Shin LM, Coffey BJ, et al. (2001) Probing striato-thalamic function in obsessive-compulsive disorder and Tourette syndrome using neuroimaging methods. Adv Neurol 85: 207–224.
  68. 68. Stein DJ, Liu Y, Shapira NA, Goodman WK (2001) The psychobiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder: how important is the role of disgust? Curr Psychiatry Rep 3: 281–287.
  69. 69. Stein DJ, Goodman WK, Rauch SL (2000) The cognitive-affective neuroscience of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2: 341–346.
  70. 70. Carter CS, Braver TS, Barch DM, Botvinick MM, Noll D, et al. (1998) Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science 280: 747–749.
  71. 71. Coles MGH, Scheffers MK, Holroyd CB (2001) Why is there an ERN/Ne on correct trials? Response representations, stimulus-related components, and the theory of error-processing. Biological Psychology 56: 173–189.